Color, flavor, and texture are the three
principal quality attributes that determine food acceptance, and
color has a far greater influence on our judgment than most of us
appreciate. We use color to determine if a banana is at our
preferred ripeness level, and a discolored meat product can warn us
that the product may be spoiled. The marketing departments of our
food corporations know that, for their customers, the color must be
“right.” The University of California Davis scorecard for wine
quality designates 4 points out of 20, or 20 % of the total score,
for color and appearance [1]. Food scientists who establish
quality control specifications for their product are very aware of
the importance of color and appearance. While subjective visual
assessment and use of visual color standards are still used in the
food industry, instrumental color measurements are extensively
employed. Objective measurement of color is desirable for both
research and industrial applications, and the ruggedness,
stability, and ease of use of today’s color measurement instruments
have resulted in their widespread adoption.
Color can be defined as the sensation
that is experienced by an individual when radiant energy within the
visible spectrum (380–770 nm) falls upon the retina of the eye
[2], and a colorant is a pigment that is used to
color a product. For the phenomenon of color to occur, there must
be: (1) a colored object, (2) light in the visible region of the
spectrum, and (3) an observer. All three of these factors must be
taken into account when assessing and measuring color. When white
light strikes an object, it can be absorbed, reflected, and/or
scattered. Selective absorption of certain wavelengths of light is
the primary basis for the color of an object. Color, as seen by the
eye, is an interpretation by the brain of the character of light
coming from an object. Colorimetryis the science of color
measurement [3]. It is possible to define color in
mathematical units; however, those numbers do not easily relate to
the observed color. A number of color-ordering systems and color
spaces have been developed that better agree with visual
assessment. In food research and quality control, instruments are
needed which provide repeatable data that correspond to how the eye
sees color. This chapter will provide a brief description of human
physiology of vision and an overview of the different
color-ordering and color-measuring systems. The chapter is limited
to presenting the basic underlying principles that will hopefully
allow for an understanding of how color of food products should be
measured. Color measurement is a very complex subject, and for more
detailed exploration of the subject, the following references are
recommended [2–7].
31.2
Physiological Basis of Color
Humans have excellent color perception
and they can detect up to 10,000,000 different colors [8].
They have very poor color memory, however, and cannot accurately
recall colors of objects previously observed [5,
9], hence the need for objective measurement of
color. While color perception varies somewhat with humans, it is
much less variable than that for the senses of taste and smell.
Color perception is comparatively uniform for people with normal
color vision; however, 8 % of males and 0.5 % of females have
physiological defects and perceive colors in a markedly different
way [2, 5].
Figure 31.1 is a simplified
diagram of the human eye. Light enters the eye through the cornea,
passes through the aqueous and vitreous humor, and is focused on
the retina, which
contains the receptor system [10].
The macula is a small
(approximately 5 mm in diameter) and highly sensitive part of the
retina that is responsible for detailed central vision. It is
located roughly in the center of the retina. It is yellow-orange
colored and contains a high concentration of the carotenoid
pigments, lutein and zeaxanthin. It is believed that these dietary
antioxidants may protect the retina from photo damage [11].
Age-related macular degeneration results in loss of central vision
and is a major health issue in our aging population. The
fovea, the very center of
the macula, is about 2 mm in diameter and contains a high
concentration of cones,
which are responsible for daylight and color vision, known as
“photopic” vision. The
cones contain receptors that are sensitive to red, green, and blue
light. Figure 31.2 shows the spectral sensitivity curves for
the three respective cones. Rods are more widely distributed in the
retina and are sensitive to low-intensity light. They have no color
discrimination and are responsible for night or “scotopic” vision. Figure 31.3 shows the spectral
sensitivity curves for scotopic (rod) and photopic (cone) vision,
the latter being an integration of the curves shown in Fig.
31.2. Note the
sensitivity maximum is at 510 nm for scotopic vision and 580 nm for
photopic vision. This accounts for blues appearing to be brighter
and reds darker at twilight when both scotopic vision and photopic
vision are functioning.
Signals are sent via the optic nerve to
the brain, where “vision” occurs. According to the “Color Opponent
Theory” [4], the signals from the red, green, and blue
receptors are transformed to one brightness signal indicating
darkness and lightness and two hue signals, red vs. green and blue
vs. yellow. Figure 31.4 shows a diagram of the opponent color
model. The brain’s interpretation of signals is a complex
phenomenon and is influenced by a variety of psychological aspects.
One such aspect is color
constancy. The same sheet of white paper will appear white
when seen in bright sunlight and also when it is viewed indoors
under dim light. The physical stimuli in each case are obviously
quite different, but the brain knows that the paper should be white
and draws on its experience. A second aspect occurs when a large
expanse of color appears brighter than the same color in a small
area. One only needs the experience of painting a whole wall of a
room and then seeing how different it appears from the small color
chip obtained from the paint store.
31.3 Color
Specification Systems
There are verbal, visual matching, and
instrumental methods for describing and specifying color. Color is
three-dimensional, and any color-order system will need to
addresshue, what we
instinctively think of as color (e.g., red, blue, green);
value, which represents
lightness and darkness; and chroma or saturation which indicates intensity.
When attempting to verbally describe a color defect or problem, one
should attempt to use these three qualities in formulating a color
description.
31.3.1
Visual Systems
The Munsell system is probably the best
known and most widely used visual color-ordering system. It was
developed by A.H. Munsell, a Boston art teacher, in 1905. In this
system red, yellow, green, blue, and purple plus five adjacent
pairs, green yellow, yellow red, red purple, purple blue, and blue
green, describe hue.
Value is that quality of color described by lightness and
darkness, from white to grey to black. Value is designated from 0
(absolute black) to 10 (absolute white). Chroma is that quality that describes
the extent a color differs from a gray of the same value. It is
designated in increasing numbers starting with 0 (neutral grey) and
extending to /16 or even higher. A change from pink to red is an
example of an increase in chroma. In Munsell notation, hue is
listed first and designated by a number and letter combination.
Numbers run from 1 to 100, and the letters are taken from the ten
major hue names, e.g., 10 GY. Value follows with a number from 0 to
10 followed by a slash mark, which is followed by a number for
chroma (e.g., 5R 5/10).
One of Munsell’s objectives was to
develop a system based on equal visual perception, with equal steps
of perception for each of the coordinates. For example, the
difference in value between 2 and 3 is visually equivalent to the
difference between 5 and 6. This visual linearity applies to the
other coordinates as well. The Munsell systems’ visual linearity
undoubtedly contributes to its success and wide popularity in many
different fields. Figure 31.5 illustrates the Munsell color system,
showing a circle of hues at value 5 and chroma 6, the neutral
values from 0 to 10, and the chroma of purple-blue (5PB) at value
5. The ten named hues are shown with additional intermediate hues
interspersed. The distance from the core to the edge shows
increasing chroma, the maximum chroma differing considerably for
different hues (e.g., R5 has a maximum of 12 and yellow has a
maximum of 6). Interactive kits that demonstrate the relationships
between Munsell hue, value, and chroma are available for purchase
[12]. Also available is the Munsell Book of Color
with 1,605 colored chips, each with a numerical designation.
Assessing color of foods by visual
comparison with color standards is an option for a number of food
products. USDA color standards are available for honey, frozen
French fried potatoes, peanut butter, and canned ripe olives, for
example [12]. This method is simple, convenient, and easy
to understand; however, it is subjective.
31.3.2
Instrumental Measurement of Color
31.3.2.1
Historical Development
For a more detailed discussion of the
historical development, refer to the 4th edition of this text
[14]. The CIE (Commission Internationale de l ’
Eclairage, or the
International Commission on
Illumination) is the main international organization
concerned with color and color measurement [3].
Standard illuminants for color measurement were first established
in 1931 by the CIE. Figure 31.6 shows the spectral power distribution
curves of three standard CIE illuminants, A, C, and D65.
Illuminant C was adopted
in 1931 and represents overcast daylight, while illuminant D65, which was
adopted in 1965, also represents average daylight but includes the
ultraviolet wavelength region. Illuminant A, adopted in 1931, represents
an incandescent light bulb. Objects will appear to have different
colors when viewed under illuminants A and C. Because of the
predominance of long wavelength light and lesser amounts of shorter
wavelength light of illuminant A, one can predict that objects will
appear to have a “warmer” color under illuminant A than under other
illuminants. Metamerism
occurs when two objects appear to have the same color under one
light source but exhibit different colors under another source.
Scientists knew that a color sensation
could be matched by mixing three colored lights [3].
W.D. Wright in 1928 and J. Guild in 1931 conducted independent
experiments in which people with normal color vision visually
matched spectral (single wavelength) light by mixing different
amounts of three primary lights (red, green, and blue) using
rheostats (Fig. 31.7). The process was repeated for test colors
covering the entire visible spectrum. The field of view for these experiments is
described as 2°, which is
similar to viewing a dime at an arm’s length. The purpose of these
viewing conditions was to have primary involvement of the fovea,
the retinal area of greatest visual acuity. The red, green, and
blue response factors were averaged and mathematically converted to
x, y, and z functions that quantify the red, green,
and blue cone sensitivity of the average human observer. The
observer functions were standardized and adopted by CIE in 1931 as
the CIE 2° standard color observer. The standard
observer curves provide human sensory response factors that are
used in color measurement worldwide (Fig. 31.8). Subsequently it was
realized that more realistic data could be obtained from a larger
field of view. The experiment was repeated using a 10° field of
view and adopted by CIE in 1964 as the 10 ° standard observer. Both sets of data
are used today, but the 10° standard observer is preferable because
it better correlates to visual assessments.
31.3.2.2 The
CIE Tristimulus System
With the adoption of standard observer
functions and standard illuminants, it became possible to convert
the spectral transmission or reflectance curve of any object to
three numerical values. These numbers are known as the
CIE tristimulus values,
X, Y, and Z, the amounts of red, green, and blue
primaries required to give a color match. The data values for a
standard illuminant and the standard observer functions are
multiplied by the % reflectance or % transmission values for the
object at selected wavelengths. Summation of the products for the
wavelengths in the visible spectrum (essentially integrating the
areas under the three curves) gives the resulting X, Y,
and Z tristimulus values. This
can mathematically be represented as follows:
(31.1)
(31.2)
(31.3)
where:
R =
sample spectrum
E =
source light spectrum
= standard observer curves.
With the objective of plotting the
three coordinates in two dimensions, the CIE converted the
X, Y, and Z tristimulus values to x, y,
and z coordinates by the
following mathematical operation:
(31.4)
(31.5)
(31.6)
Since x + y + z
= 1, only two coordinates are needed to describe color as
z = 1−(x + y).
Figure 31.9 shows the 1931
chromaticity diagram
where x vs. y are plotted to give the
horseshoe-shaped locus. Spectral colors lie around the perimeter
and white light (illuminant D65) has the coordinates
x = 0.314, y = 0.331. With the aid of a ruler, a
line can be drawn from the coordinates for white light through the
object coordinates to the edge, which gives the dominant wavelength, λd. Dominant wavelength is analogous
to hue in the Munsell system. The distance from the white light
coordinates to the object coordinates, relative to the distance
from the white light coordinates to λd, is described as
% purity and is analogous
to chroma in the Munsell system. The standard observer curve for y
(green) shown in Fig. 31.8 is very similar to the sensitivity curve
for human photopic vision shown in Fig. 31.3. Because of this,
tristimulus value Y is known as
luminosity and is analogous to
value in the Munsell system.
Manual calculation of XYZ tristimulus values from
reflectance/transmission spectra is a tedious operation. Modern
colorimetric spectrophotometers measure the light reflected or
transmitted from an object, and the data are sent to a processor
where it is multiplied by standard illuminant and standard observer
functions to give the XYZ
tristimulus values. Since objects with identical XYZ tristimulus values will provide a
color match, they find application in the paper, paint, and textile
industries. Unfortunately, the XYZ numbers do not easily relate to
observed color, and they have the limitation of not having
equivalent visual spacing. [Referral to Fig. 31.9 reveals that the
wavelength spacing in the green region (500–540 nm) is much larger
than that in the red (600–700 nm) or blue (380–480 nm) regions.]
The same numerical color differences between colors will not equate
to the same visual difference for all colors. This is a severe
limitation in measurement of color of food products, as major
interest is in how food product color deviates from a standard or
changes during processing and storage. Statistical analysis of
color data for which numerical units were nonequivalent would be
problematic.
31.3.3
Tristimulus Colorimeters and Color Spaces
Richard S. Hunter, Deane B. Judd, and
Henry A. Gardner were among the pioneering scientists who in the
1940s were working to develop color-measuring instruments that
would overcome the disadvantages of the CIE spectrophotometric
tristimulus system [2, 5,
6]. Light sources that were similar to illuminant
C were used, along with filter systems that approximated the
sensitivity of the cones in the human eye. Empirical approaches
were taken to get more equivalent visual spacing. In an effort to
get numerical values that better related to observed color, a
system that applied the color opponent theory of color perception
was developed [3].
The Hunter color solid (Fig. 31.10) was first
published in 1942 where L
indicated lightness; a, the red (+) or green (−) coordinate; and b, the yellow (+) or blue (−) coordinate. The Hunter L a b color space has been widely
adopted by the food industry. It is very effective for measuring
color differences. The Lab
system was subsequently improved to give more uniform color
spacing. In 1976, the CIE officially adopted the modified system as
CIELAB with the
parameters L*a*b*.
L* indicates lightness (0–100) with 0 being black
and 100 being white. The coordinate a* is for red (+) and green (−), and b* is for yellow (+) and blue (−). The limits for a* and b* are approximately + or − 80. Figure
31.11 shows a
portion of the a*, b* chromaticity diagram where a* and b* are both positive, representing a
color range from red to yellow. Point A is the plot of a* and b* for a red apple. The angle from the
start of the +a* axis to point
A can be calculated as arctanb*/a* and is known as hue angle, h or H*. The distance from the center to point
A is chroma, which is
calculated as the hypotenuse of the right triangle formed by the
origin and the values of coordinates a and b. . The CIE has also recommended
adoption of this color scale known as CIELCH or L*C*H*.
This color space (which is illustrated in Fig. 31.12) designates hue
(H*) as one of the three
dimensions, the other two being lightness (L*) and chroma (C*), which have an obvious parallel to
Munsell hue, value, and chroma. This color space is advantageous as
hue is most critical to humans with normal color vision for
perception and acceptability. In this system, 0° represents red,
90°—yellow, 180°—green, and 270°—blue. Figure 31.13 shows plots of
a* and b* for three hypothetical objects having
the following a*b* coordinates: a* = +12 and b* = +8; a* = +12 and b* = +4; a* = +12 and b* = −4. While all objects have identical
a* values, their colors range
from purplish red (H* = 342°)
to red (H* = 18°) to orange
(H* = 34°). A common error in
interpretation of color measurements is to use only the coordinate
a* as a measure of “redness.”
Monitoring color change is more understandable if one measures
lightness (L*), hue angle
(H* from 0 to 360°), and
chroma. Chroma will increase with increasing pigment concentration
and then decrease as the sample becomes darker. Thus, it is
possible for one light and one dark sample to have the same hue
angle and the same chroma. They will readily be distinguished,
however, because of their different L* values.
The colorimeters that are available in
the market today have vastly improved from earlier models with
respect to stability, ruggedness, and ease of use. There are
handheld instruments that are portable for use in the field, online
instruments for process control, and specialized colorimeters for
specific commodities. They vary with respect to operating in
transmission or reflectance mode and size of sample viewing area.
Colorimeters have a high degree of precision, but do not have a
high degree of accuracy with respect to identifying or matching
colors. Most colorimeters used in research are color
spectrophotometers with a diffraction grating for scanning the
visible spectrum, with the data being sent to a microprocessor for
conversion of reflectance or transmission data to tristimulus
numbers. In operating the instrument, choices must be made as to
illuminant, viewing angle (2° or 10°), and data
presentation as XYZ ,
Lab, CIEL*a*b*,
or L*C*H*.
Illuminant D65, 10° viewing angle, and L*C*H*
are appropriate for most food applications. It should be obvious
that different numbers will be obtained with different illuminants,
viewing angles, and color scales. It is critical that the
illuminant, viewing angle, and color scale used in color
measurement be specified in technical reports and research
publications.
31.4
Practical Considerations in Color Measurement
Choice of an appropriate instrument,
sample preparation, sample presentation, and handling of data are
issues that must be dealt with in color measurement.
31.4.1
Interaction of Light with Sample
When a sample is illuminated with
light, a number of things occur that are illustrated in Fig.
31.14. Light
for which the angle of reflection is equal to the angle of
incidence is described as specular
light. Smooth polished surfaces will appear glossy because of the high degree of
specular reflection. Rough
surfaces will have a great deal of diffuse reflection and will have a dull
or matte appearance. Selective
absorption of light will result in the appearance of color.
Opaque samples will reflect light. Transparent samples will primarily
transmit light, and
translucent samples will
both reflect and
transmit light. Ideal samples
for color measurement will be flat, smooth, uniform, matte, and
either opaque or transparent. A brick of colored Cheddar cheese is
one of the few food examples that come close to having those
characteristics.
31.4.2
Instrument Choice
Instrument geometry refers to the
arrangement of light
source, sample
placement, and detector. The CIE recognizes the
following instrument geometries: 45°/0° where the specimen is illuminated at
45° and measured at 0° and, the inverse, 0°/45° where the specimen is illuminated at
0° and measured at 45°. Diffuse reflectance is measured since
specular light is excluded. These are illustrated in Fig.
31.15.
Diffuse sphere geometry
is the third type where a white-coated sphere is used to illuminate
a sample. With some sphere geometry instruments, measurements can
either include or exclude specular reflectance. These instruments
are versatile in that they can measure in transmission for
transparent samples and in reflectance for opaque samples. Some can
also measure the amount of light scattering, turbidity or haze in
liquid samples, and the amount of gloss in solid samples.
Instruments with 45°/0° and 0°/45° geometries can only measure
reflectance.
31.4.3 Color
Difference Equations and Color Tolerances
When colorimeter measurements are
conducted under carefully controlled conditions, data with a high
degree of precision can be obtained. In both industrial and
research applications, the interest is primarily in how color
dimensions deviate from a standard or how they change from batch to
batch, year to year, or during processing and storage. Color
differences are calculated by subtracting L*a*b*
and L*C*H*
values for the sample from the standard, e.g.,Delta L
* = L*sample – L*standard. Positive
∆L* numbers will be lighter
than the standard, and negative ΔL* numbers will be darker.
Delta
a* = a*sample –
a*standard. Positive
Δa* numbers will be more “red”
(or less “green”) than the standard, and negative Δa* numbers will be more “green” (or less
“red”).
Delta b* = b*sample – b*standard. Positive
∆b* numbers will be more
“yellow” (or less “blue”), and negative Δb* numbers will be more “blue” (or less
“yellow”).
Delta C* = C*sample – C*standard. Positive
∆C* numbers mean the sample has
greater intensity or is more saturated, and negative ∆C* numbers mean that the sample is less
saturated.
Delta
H* = H*sample –
H*standard. Positive
H* numbers indicate the hue
angle is in the counterclockwise direction from the standard, and
negative numbers are in the clockwise direction. If the standard
has a hue angle of 90°, a positive ∆H* is a shift in the green direction,
and a negative ∆H* number is a
shift in the red direction.
A single number is often desired in
industry for establishing pass/fail acceptability limits.
Total color difference
(∆E*) is calculated by the
following equation:
(31.7)
A limitation of ∆E* is that the
single number will only indicate the magnitude of color difference,
not the direction. Samples with identical ∆E* numbers will not necessarily have the
same visual appearance.
In establishing color tolerances,
∆L*, ∆C*, and ∆H* numbers are preferred since they
correlate well with visual appearance. A diagram showing acceptable
tolerances based on ∆L*,
∆C*, and ∆H* numbers is shown in Fig. 31.16. The elliptical
shape of the solid arises since tolerances for ∆H* are considerably narrower than for
∆C* and ∆L*.
31.4.4
Sample Preparation and Presentation
For color measurement data to be at all
useful, the numbers must be consistent and repeatable. Sampling of
product must be done so that it is representative of the product
and prepared so that it represents the product’s color
characteristics. Many food samples are far from ideal in that they
may be partially transmitting and partially reflecting. Rather than
being uniform, they may be mottled or highly variable in color. The
number of readings that need to be taken for acceptable
repeatability is dependent on the nature of the sample. Another
problem is that often the only instrument available is one that is
less than ideal for the sample. Gordon Leggett [15]
provides some practical tips and a systematic protocol for
consistent color measurement of different food categories.
Transparent liquids should be measured with a sphere instrument,
using a clear glass or plastic cell. A cell filled with distilled
water can be used as a blank to negate the effects of cell and
solvent. Cell path length is selected based on color intensity. A
20 mm cell is used for most colored liquids, with 10 mm cells for
highly absorbing liquids. A very thin 2 mm cell may be appropriate
for highly absorbent transparent liquids such as soy sauce. For
nearly colorless liquids, a 50 mm cell may be necessary. For clear
transparent liquids, a single measurement using a viewing area of
15 mm diameter or greater may be sufficient for good repeatability.
For hazy transparent liquids, two to four readings with replacement
of the liquid between readings is necessary to get acceptable
repeatability when using a 10 mm path length cell and a sphere
instrument.
Liquid samples with high solids are
translucent rather than transparent. They can be measured by
transmission using a very thin 2 mm path length cell, or measured
in reflectance. Here it is necessary to control the thickness of
the sample so that it is effectively opaque. Solid foods vary with
respect to size, geometry, and uniformity. With some colorimeters,
reflectance measurements can be taken directly on the sample.
Ideally the surface should be flat. Readings of an apple or orange
may be distorted because of the “pillowing” effect, which is a
result of the distorted reflectance values from the uneven surface.
Pureeing nonuniform materials such as strawberries will give a
uniform sample; however, the incorporation of air renders a color
extremely different from the sample of interest. For opaque foods,
instruments with 45*/0° and 0°/45° geometries are recommended as
the measurements correlate better with visual assessment than those
obtained with sphere instruments. Instruments with a large area of
view, e.g., 25–50 mm, are helpful for area-averaging nonuniform
color. For powders, two readings with replacement of the powder
between readings may be sufficient, but for flakes, chunks, and
large particulates, a large field of view (40 mm or larger) with
three to six readings and sample replacement between readings is
recommended.
Different commodities present their own
peculiarities when it comes to measuring color and appearance. In
the proceedings of an American Chemical Society symposium
[16], various authors discuss methodology for
color measurement of meat, fish, wine, beer, and several fruits and
vegetables.
31.5
Summary
Color is three-dimensional, and any
color-ordering or color-measuring system needs to address that
fact. The Munsell system is a visual system that designates color
in terms of hue, value, and chroma. Each of these dimensions has
equivalent visual spacing, which is advantageous. The physiology of
color vision has been long understood, and it provided the
necessary background information for development of the CIE
tristimulus system. Standardization of illuminants and experiments
using humans with normal color version was necessary to develop
color-matching functions that corresponded to the color sensitivity
of the human eye. The system permits calculation of numerical
XYZ tristimulus values that can
accurately represent a color and are useful in color matching. The
system does not have equivalent visual spacing, which is a
disadvantage when measuring how a sample differs from a standard or
changes during processing and storage. Color-order systems have
been developed that are more suitable for measuring color
differences. These include the HunterLab system, the CIEL*a*b*
system, and the L*C*H*
system. The latter two systems are recommended by the CIE, the
International Association with responsibility for standardization
and measurement of light. They have been widely adopted by the food
industry for color measurement. There are many colorimeters
available for industrial and research applications that are rugged,
easy to standardize, and user friendly. They vary with respect to
presentation of sample, size of viewing area, portability, and the
ability to measure by transmittance or reflectance. Many food
samples are less than ideal for color measurements because they may
be partial transmitting and partial reflecting, nonuniform, and of
varying size and shape. A number of factors need to be considered
with respect to sample preparation and presentation to get
measurements that are repeatable and that correspond to visual
appearance.
There are a number of excellent
illustrative tutorials dealing with color measurement that are
available on various websites that have been developed by
organizations and commercial companies. The following are
recommended: HunterLab [17], Konica Minolta [18],
CIE [19], Munsell [20],
Color Models Technical Guides [21], A
Review of RGB Color Spaces [22], and Beer Color Laboratories
[23].
31.6 Study
Questions
1.
Dominant wavelength (λd), %
purity, and luminosity (Y) in the CIE XYZ system correspond to what indices in
the Munsell system? In the CIE L*C*H*
system?
2.
Using a calculator, determine hue angle
and chroma for the following sets of a*, b* data: a* = +12 and b* = +8, a* = +12 and b* = +4, and a* = 12 and b* = -4.
3.
If one wants to use a colorimeter to
measure of the amount of browning in maple syrup, what indices
would you expect to correspond well with visual assessment?
4.
How variable is human color perception
when compared with that of taste and smell? What are the human
capabilities for color perception and color memory?
5.
Why is CIE tristimulus Y used as a
measure of luminosity?
6.
Give examples where it is appropriate to
use a colorimeter with diffuse sphere geometry and, conversely, a
colorimeter with 0°/45° reflectance geometry.
7.
How can you determine how many readings
should be taken for a given sample?
Acknowledgment
The authors of this chapter wish to
acknowledge Dr. Jack Francis, a legend in the area of color
analysis and the person who wrote the chapter on this topic in two
previous editions of this book. Ideas for the content or
organization, along with some of the text, came from his chapter.
Dr. Francis offered the use of his chapter contents.
Open
Access This chapter is licensed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 2.5 International
License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.5/), which
permits any noncommercial use, sharing, adaptation, distribution
and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give
appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons license and indicate if
changes were made.
The images or other third party material in
this chapter are included in the chapter's Creative Commons
license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the chapter's Creative
Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory
regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain
permission directly from the copyright holder.