Margaret Semrud-Clikeman and
Phyllis Anne Teeter Ellison
Child NeuropsychologyAssessment and Interventions for Neurodevelopmental
Disorders2

Margaret Semrud-Clikeman
Michigan State University, Lansing,
MI, USA
Phyllis Anne Teeter Ellison
Department of Educational Psychology,
University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI, USA
ISBN 978-0-387-88962-7e-ISBN 978-0-387-88963-4
Library of Congress Control
Number: 2008942517
© Springer-Verlag US 2009
First softcover printing 2007
All rights
reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in
part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer
Science+Business Media, LLC, 233 Spring Street, New York, NY 10013,
USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or
scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information
storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or
by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter
developed is forbidden.
The use in this
publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks, and similar
terms, even if they are not identified as such, is not to be taken
as an expression of opinion as to whether or not they are subject
to proprietary rights.
Printed on acid-free
paper
springer.com
Foreword
The human brain represents
the product of an ongoing, six-billion-year construction project.
In its physical form and function, the human brain represents
millions upon millions of trial-and-error adaptive adjustments.
Comprised of an estimated 100 billion neurons and many more glial
cells it is organized into thousands of regions. The human brain,
in a seamlessly integrated manner, governs body functions and
movement but more importantly, regulates cognition. Not
surprisingly, although the brains of different animals may not look
exactly alike, they all work according to the same principles and
mechanisms. These neurons and glial cells communicate using a
nearly infinite number of synaptic connections, yet the entire
organ in humans weighs only about three pounds. As authors Sandra
Aamodt and Sam Wang eloquently describe in their book, Welcome to your brain (2007), billions
of years of evolution have resulted in a very complex human brain,
yet one that is a jumbled, far from efficient, crowded organ. They
describe the neuronal pathways of the human brain as the equivalent
of the New York City subway system or the streets of London with
layers upon layers of routes each constructed at a different time
in a different way. Yet this stunning system, for the most part,
develops and works fine for most children.
The adult human brain at between 1300 and 1400 cm
is by far not the largest brain among mammals. Consider that a
sperm whale's brain is approximately 7800 cm and an elephant's
brain is 4700 cm. Thus, bigger brains alone do not necessarily mean
smarter or more developed organisms. Although larger brains are
associated with higher intelligence to some extent, smaller brains
can be advantageous from an evolutionary point of view,
particularly if they are equal in intelligence to larger brains.
But many additional factors beyond brain size impact intelligence.
Brain size in vertebrates such as humans may relate more to social
rather than mechanical skills. Lower ratios of brain to body mass
may increase the amount of brain mass available for more complex
cognitive tasks. For reptiles it is about 1:1500; for birds, 1:220;
for most mammals, 1:180 and for humans, 1:50. MRI studies of humans
have demonstrated that to some extent brain size has modest
correlation with intelligence. Among our ancestors, homo erectus
had a brain size of about 980 cm; homo habilis a brain of about 750
cm; homo floresiensis a brain size of about 380 cm; and
neanderthals a brain size slightly larger than our current brains.
Consider also that an infant is born with a brain of 300–400 cm
tripling in size by the adult years. Yet, between birth and the
conclusion of the first two decades of life, a nearly infinite
acquisition of knowledge and behaviors characterizes human
development. Gram for gram the human brain delivers an almost
dazzling array of motoric, behavioral, cognitive and emotional
capacities nearly impossible to fathom in light of its' size. The
brain is a metabolically high cost organ consuming about 20% of the
body's metabolic energy providing further evidence of its' complex
operations. Further most energy use is devoted to being ready to
think and respond rather than thinking per se.
Despite rapid and fascinating advances in our
understanding of brain structure, function and complex human
behavior, it still remains the case that there is much more that we
don't know about how the brain grows, functions and ages. Though
neuroimaging techniques have allowed scientists to appreciate the
relationship between the anatomy and physiology of the brain and
motor functions for example, the basic cognitive operations of the
brain remain elusive.
Beyond anatomical structure
and physiology, the brain unlike any other organ in the body
creates an alter ego, the conscious mind. In his fictional short
story They're made out of
meat, author Terry Bison describes aliens with electronic
brains referring to humans as “thinking meat”! The idea that the
brain can create consciousness seems like a science fiction
phenomena. For thousands of years philosophers and scientists have
debated and waxed poetic about the nature of the human mind. The
mind appears to be composed of a set of processes driven by
language, organized by memory and individualized by each person's
unique perception and interpretation of their lives. And yet, the
human brain does not appear to possess a localized center of
conscious control. Though highly dependent upon the frontal lobes,
consciousness is also dependent upon sensory, processing and
interpretative abilities distributed throughout the brain. A
description of the biological basis of human consciousness
continues to elude the best efforts of current researchers. We
understand how people lose a sense of consciousness.We also
understand how certain conditions are created by alterations in the
brain and conscious experience. Yet as we come to appreciate and
understand the relationship between certain conscious activities
and structures within the brain, it still remains the case that our
consciousness extends well beyond the structures and physiology of
our brain.
In her extremely cogent and
interesting book, Brain
dance (2004), Dean Falk, a professor of anthropology at
Florida State University, describes the conditions and
circumstances that allowed a group of ape-like individuals to
evolve over a period of at least 5 million years into homo sapiens.
During this process, the brain became increasingly more specialized
evolving a broad range of abilities as well as
right-brain/left-brain and male/female differences. As Falk notes,
in less than 2 million years, brain size doubled in the homo
species, from around 650 to 1350 cm. Only a small portion of this
newly evolved, larger brain was tied to increasing body size. As
Falk points out, this process was unprecedented in the evolutionary
histories of other mammals. As brain size increased, neurons
enlarged and became more widely spaced and the cerebral cortex
became more convoluted. As Falk notes, no new structures were found
in these larger human brains.However these larger brains set the
foundation for an accelerated evolutionary process never before
witnessed in any earthbound, mammalian species. In this process,
the pre-frontal cortex and the posterior areas of the brain
associated with sensory processing in particular became especially
convoluted. As Falk points out, the shift in neurochemicals,
anatomy of neurons and brain function provided the underlying
mechanics of our rapid evolutionary progression, a pattern that was
most certainly driven by natural selection.
It is also the case that for hundreds of
thousands if not millions of years our ancestors have developed a
finely tuned capacity to respond emotionally to events in the
environment leading neuronal pathways between emotive centers of
the brain stem and cerebral motor control areas to be shorter than
those connecting complex, cognitive areas in the frontal lobes.
Though as human beings we still process the world first and
foremost emotionally, we have developed an impressive capacity to
think before acting on emotion alone. Yet it is still the case that
stressful experiences may quickly override our capacity for
rationale, reflective responding.
Finally, out of this amazing
progression forward, we developed language. As Clive Bromhill
writes in his book, The eternal
child (2003), long after we developed the ability to walk on
two legs and our brains became larger than those of any other
species on the planet, we were still limited in our capacity for
complex thinking. For a long period of time, our ancestors' brains
grew larger but we appeared to reap few intellectual benefits.
However, within the past 50,000 years something happened in the
human brain that transformed the already large brains of our
ancestors into what they are today. At some point, brain circuitry
changed. Our human ancestors through the harnessing of language
developed the ability to think. As Bromhall notes, the brain became
partitioned, permitting the capacity for subjective experience. In
other words, we can simultaneously experience internal thoughts and
the external world, a key ingredient in consciousness.
As the center of our consciousness and being, it
is fitting that we devote an increasing scientific literature to
understanding and facilitating the operation of the developing
brain; in particular an appreciation of the developmental disorders
and conditions that adversely affect children's transition into
adulthood. Children's brains represent an incredible capacity to
learn. In the span of 18 months between 1 1/2 and 3 years of age
for example, children move from not speaking to telling us how to
live our lives! In bilingual homes they master two languages
simultaneously.
Clinical child neuropsychologists today and in
the future must be scientist practitioners. To do so effectively
requires a special type of literature at our fingertips. The first
edition of Clinical Child Neuropsychology provided such an
essential resource. As research scientists the joint and individual
work of Drs. Ann Teeter and Margaret Semrud-Clikeman over the past
thirty years has greatly expanded the boundaries of brain
neuroscience. I have had the exceptional opportunity to work
professionally with both of them. In the second edition of this
seminal work, Drs. Ann Teeter and Margaret Semrud-Clikeman have
authored a number of new chapters, included case studies in all
chapters and completely re-written and updated existing chapters.
This volume seamlessly blends current knowledge in pediatric
neuroscience with practical, reasoned and reasonable strategies to
understand, evaluate and treat the myriad of neurodevelopmental
problems children experience as they grow into adulthood. With
great pleasure and admiration I will place this volume next to the
first edition of this text on my bookshelf.
References
Aamodt, S. A., & Wang,
S. (2007) Welcome to your
brain. New York: Bloomsbury Press.
Bromhill, C. (2003)
The eternal child. London:
Ebury Press.
Falk, D. (2004).
Brain dance (Revised and
Expanded Edition). Gainesville, FL: University Press.
Contents
Part I Anatomy and
Physiology
IDEA 8
References 20
Part II Clinical Assessment
References 176
Part III Childhood and Adolescent
Disorders
Primary Symptoms 189
Genetic Factors 192
Family Factors 194
Conclusion 204
Attention 209
Academic 209
Recommendations 211
Attention 216
Academic 216
Recommendations 218
References 219
Definitions 281
Genetic Factors 285
Summary 297
Summary 300
Genetic Factors 302
Chapter Summary 316
References 316
Treatment 399
Family Issues 401
Chapter Summary 405
References 406
Part IV An Integration Intervention
Paradigm
16 Neuropsychological
Intervention and Treatment Approaches for Childhood and Adolescent
Disorders 413
Summary 430
References 430
Erratum E1
Author Index 453
Subject Index 477