GIVE HIM SOMETHING TO DO
One of the best ways to see your dog's
capacities and interests is simply to provide a lot of possible
things to interact with. Wiggle a string in front of your dog's
nose along the ground; stash a treat in a shoebox; or invest in the
many creative dog toys that are marketed. A rich set of things to
burrow into, nose, chew, bob, shake, pursue, or watch will engage
your dog—and keep him from finding his own burrowable and chewable
objects among your possessions. Outside, agility training or some
simulacrum obstacle course is a well-defined way to engage and
interest many energetic but driven dogs. But interest can be spiked
simply by a weaving, smell-laden path, or the unexplored reaches of
a field.
Dogs like both the familiar and the new.
Happiness is novelty—new toys, new treats—in a safe, well-known
place. It can be cure for boredom, too: the new requires attention
and prompts activity. Hiding food to be searched for is one
example: they must move around to explore the space, using nose and
paw and mouth together. You need only watch the exuberance of an
agility dog on a new course to see how good new is.
PLAY WITH HIM
In youth, but even throughout their lives,
dogs are constantly learning about the world, like the developing
child. Games that children find mind-bogglingly fun work with dogs
too. Peekaboo, disappearing around the corner or under a blanket
instead of behind hands, is especially fun when dogs are learning
about invisible displacement, that objects continue to exist when
you can no longer see them. Dogs are astute perceivers of
associations, and you can play with that: ring the bell before
dinner, Ivan Pavlov found, and the dogs anticipate dinner. You can
connect bells—or horns, whistles, harmonica, gospel music, just
about anything—not just with food, but with people arriving, or the
time for a bath. Make a string of associations—and treat your dog's
actions as adding to that string. Play imitation games, mirroring
what your dog does: jumping on the bed, yelping, pawing the air.
Note your dog's current skills, and try to stretch his ability. If
he seems to know walk or ball, start using words that make more subtle
distinctions: smell walk and
blue ball; evening smell walk and
blue squeaky ball. And at any age, play
with your dog as a dog would. Choose your play signal—play-slap
your hands on the ground, mimic panting close to his face, race
away with looks back at him—and play. Treat your hands as he does
his mouth and grab head, legs, tail, belly. Give him a good toy to
hold on to, or be prepared for some nips. Watch as your own tail
may begin to wag.
LOOK AGAIN
Much enjoyment can be had in noticing the invisible-visible features of your dog: the things we typically see through that are on display right in front of us. We now know how attentive dogs can be to people, and to our attention: notice the various and creative methods your dog uses to try to get your attention. Does he bark or bray? Stare at you wistfully? Sigh loudly? Walk back and forth between you and the door? Lay his head on your lap? Find the methods you like, and respond to them, letting the others fade away naturally.
Notice how your dog uses his eyes; the frenzy
of his nose; how his ears fold back, prick up, and pivot toward a
distant bark. Notice all the sounds he makes, and all the sounds he
notices. Even the way the dog moves, an action so familiar as to
make him recognizable at a distance, is transformed on closer
examination: what gait does he use? A medium-sized dog may stride
forward in the classic walk, the rear
foot on one side of the body slowly chasing the front foot to the
ground, the diagonal paws moving almost in sync. Hurrying a little,
he trots, the diagonal legs now in
tandem, occasionally finding himself with only one paw of four
fully on the ground. Between the trot and the walk is the gait of
the short-legged: typical of the bulldog, front-heavy with a wide
stance, his rear end rolling as he walks. Leggy dogs do better at
the gallop, the run of greyhounds,
wherein the two rear feet precede the two front to the ground, the
dog's body alternating between outstretched, and airborne and
spring-loaded. In the gallop that fifth toelike digit partway up
the front leg of most dogs—the dew claw—is used for stability and
leverage; at a gallop's end you might find the usually clean dew
claw with a dollop of mud under it. Toy-sized dogs half-bound, bringing their two hind legs forward at
once but uncoupling their front footfalls. Other dogs pace, their left legs moving forward and falling at
once, followed quickly by the right. Mesmerize yourself trying to
keep track of the complexity of your dog's gait.
SPY
ON HIM
To understand what your dog's day at home
without you might be like, by all means videotape it. One of the
distinct pleasures I got with Pumpernickel was seeing her act
without me. Despite hours of videorecording, I rarely turned my
camera on to her. It was only when she didn't expect me—when a
friend had taken her out, and I arrived unannounced—that I got to
see her carry on without me.
It was spectacular to see. You can re-create
this kind of spectacle by setting up a videotape at your home when
you leave for the day. I recommend this "eavesdropping" not because
it reliably reveals spectacles—it does not—but because it allows
you to see your dog's life without you there. You will more fully
understand what his day might be like by watching a snippet of the
day pass later, minute by minute.
What I saw in my eavesdropping was Pump's
independence, freed not just from the need to check back with me,
but from the kind of scrutiny to which I subjected all her
behavior. She existed capably without me, for the hours that I
milled about in the bookstore, had an extra-long run, went
elsewhere for dinner followed by elsewhere still for drinks. This
was at once reassuring and fully humbling. I am glad that she
managed the day on her own, yet I am sometimes mystified that I
ever left her alone at all.
Most dogs are simply alone all day with little
to do, expected to wait it out until we return, and then act just
as we want them to. And we are surprised and horrified when they
actually do something in our absence! That dogs endure this (and
much worse misinterpretation and neglect) is almost part of their
constitution. We can, and do, get away with it. But dogs are
individuals. It is for this reason that they require—and
deserve—more attention to their umwelt, to their experience, to
their point of view.
DON'T BATHE YOUR DOG EVERY DAY
Let them smell like a dog as long as you can
stand it. Some dogs will even develop painful skin sores from
regular bathing. And no dog wants to smell like a bathtub that has
had a dog in it.
READ THE DOG'S TELLS
Like novice poker players, dogs reveal what
could be called their "tells"—their intent, their "hand"—with every
move, if you simply look. The configuration of the face, head,
body, and tail are all meaningful. And there is more to it than
whether the tail is wagging or the dog is barking: dogs can say
more than one thing at a time. A barking dog whose tail fans the
sky is not "about to attack" but is instead more curious, alert,
uncertain—and interested. A furiously wagging low tail undermines
the aggressiveness of a familiar dog snarling as he guards a
ball.
Given the salience of eye contact to all
canids, and the dog's use of gaze, you can get a lot of information
about an unknown dog from his eyes. Constant eye contact can be
threatening: do not approach a dog by gazing non-stop, which may be
perceived as staring him down. If he is staring at you, you can
deflect his gaze by turning away slightly, breaking eye contact.
They do the same when they are tense: turning their head to the
side, or distracting themselves with a yawn or a sudden interest in
a smell on the ground. If you think you are the recipient of a
threatening stare, you can confirm it by looking for its
accompaniment: hackles up, ears up, tail up, body frozen. A stare
with a tongue darting licks into the air is more adoring than
aggressive.
PET
FRIENDLY
Though they nearly all look pettable, not every dog likes to be petted. Attending to that is not only polite, it is sometimes imperative: a fearful or sick dog might respond to touch with aggression. There are great individual differences in dogs' sensitivity to petting, and their current interest can be changed by their state of health, their state of happiness, and past experience. For most dogs, the right touch by a human is a calming, bonding experience. Light touch is irritating or exciting; a firm hand is relaxing; too firm a hand is probably oppressive. They (and you) can be physically calmed by steady, continuous strokes from the head to rump, or by capable deep muscle massage. Watch your dog's reaction and find his preferred touch zones. And let him touch you back.
GET A MUTT
If you don't have a dog yet, or are getting
another dog, I have just the breed for you: the breedless dog, the
mutt. The myth that a shelter dog, especially a mixed-breed dog,
will be less good or less reliable than a purebred dog is not just
wrong, it is entirely backward: mixed breeds are healthier, less
anxious, and live longer than purebreds. When you buy a bred dog,
you are simply not buying a fixed object, guaranteed to act in
certain ways—regardless of what the breeder tells you. What you
might get is a dog with an overriding fixation, born of breeding
for a task that he will likely never do while living with you (who
nonetheless will still be wonderfully doggy). Mutts, on the other
hand, with the bred characteristics diluted, wind up having lots of
latent abilities and less mania.
ANTHROPOMORPHIZE WITH UMWELT IN MIND
On walks Pump was never satisfied with being
on one side of the path or the other: she weaved back and forth
capriciously. Holding her on leash I was constantly readjusting my
hand on the thing. Sometimes I'd insist she stay to one side of me,
and she sighed at me while we both glanced knowingly at the good
un-smelled spots on the other side.
Even with a scientific take on the dog, we
find ourselves using anthropomorphic words. Our dogs—my dog—make
friends, feel guilty, have fun, get jealous; understand what we
mean, think about things, know better; are sad, are happy, are
scared; want, love, hope.
This way of talking is easy, and sometimes
useful, but it is also part of a bigger, more exceptionable
phenomenon. As we recast every moment of a dog's life in human
terms, we have begun to completely lose touch with the animal in
them. It is no longer the rare dog who is shampooed, clothed in
garments, and feted on his birthday. That may seem benign, but it
is also part of a de-animalizing of dogs that is somewhat radical.
We are rarely present for their births, and many people will choose
not to be present for their own dogs' deaths. We eliminate sex for
the most part: we neuter dogs and we discourage the slightest
lascivious thrusts of the hips. They are fed sanitized food, in
bowls; they are largely restricted to a leash-length distance from
our heels. In cities, their excrement is bundled up and thrown
away. (Happily, we have not yet taught them to use the toilet …
convenient though we know that would be.) Breed types are described
like products, with specified features. It seems as though we are
trying to get rid of the animal part of the dog.
If we assume that we have reduced the animal
factor to zero, we are in for some unhappy surprises. Dogs do not
always behave just as we think they should. They may sit, lie down,
and roll over—but then will revert magnificently. They suddenly
squat and urinate in the house, bite your hand, sniff your crotch,
jump on a stranger, eat something gnarly in the grass, don't come
when you call them, roughly tackle a much smaller dog. In this way,
our frustrations with dogs often arise from our extreme
anthropomorphizing, which neglects the very animalness of dogs. A
complex animal cannot be explained simply.
The alternative to anthropomorphizing is not
simply treating animals as precisely unhuman. We now have the tools
to take a more measured look at their behavior: with their umwelt
and their perceptual and cognitive abilities in mind. Nor need we
take a dispassionate stance toward animals. Scientists
anthropomorphize … at home. They name their pets, and see love in a
named-dog's upward-turned gaze. In research, names are verboten:
while they might help tell animals apart, they are not benign.
Naming a wild animal "colors one's thinking about it forever
afterwards," a preeminent field biologist noted. There are obvious
observational biases that are introduced when you name the subject
of your observations. Jane Goodall famously violated this maxim,
and "Graybeard" became known to the world. But "Graybeard" for me
connotes a wise, old man: as a result, I may be more likely to
perceive his behavior as indicative of his wisdom than see it as
foolishness. Instead, to distinguish individual animals, most
ethologists use identifying markings—leg bands, tags, or marking
fur or feathers with dye—or look for identity in habitual behavior,
social organization, or natural physical features.*
To name a dog is to begin to make him
personal—and thus an anthropomorphizable creature. But we must. To
name a dog is to assert an interest in understanding the nature of
the dog; to not name the dog seems the pinnacle of disinterest.
Dogs named Dog make me sad: the dog is
already defined out of being a player in the owner's life.
Dog has no name of his own; he is only
a taxonomic subspecies. He will never be treated as an individual.
What one is doing when naming a dog is starting him on the
personality that he is to grow into. When trying out names for our
dog, calling words out at her—"Bean!" "Bella!"
"Blue!"—to see if any prompted a reaction, I felt that I was
searching for "her name": the name that was already hers. With it,
the bond between human and animal—wrought of understanding, not
projection—could begin to form.
Go look at your dog. Go to him! Imagine his
umwelt—and let him change your own.
Postscript: Me and My Dog
I sometimes find deep recognition in photos of
her in which her eyes were not distinguishable from the darkness of
her coat. It represents to me the way in which there was always
something mysterious about her existence to me: what it was like to
be Pump. She never laid it out there in the open. She had a privacy
about her. I feel privileged that I was let into that private
realm.
Pumpernickel wagged into my life in August
1990. We spent nearly every day together, until the day of her last
breaths in November 2006. I still spend every one of my days with
her.
Pump was a total surprise. I didn't expect to
be changed constitutionally by a dog. But it quickly became
apparent that the description "a dog" didn't capture the astounding
abundance of facets to her, the depth of her experience, and the
possibilities of a lifetime knowing her. Before long, I felt
pleasure simply at her company and pride at watching her act. She
was spirited, patient, willful, and disarming all in one great
furry bundle. She was sure of her opinions (she had no truck with
yipping dogs) and yet open to new things (as the occasional
fostered cat—despite each's unwavering disinterest). She was
effusive; she was responsive; she was great fun.
What Pump was not was a subject in my research
(at least, not intentionally). Still, I brought her with me when
going to watch dogs. She was often my passkey into dog parks, and
into dog circles: without a dog companion, a person may be treated
suspiciously by dogs and owners alike. As a result, she wanders
through many of my videos of bouts of play—through and out, as my
video camera was trained on my unwitting subjects, not on my Pump.
Now I regret my camera's unemotional oversight of her. Though I
captured the social interactions I wanted, and was eventually,
after much reviewing and analysis of the behaviors of the
interactants, able to discover some surprising abilities of dogs, I
missed some moments of my
dog.
Every dog owner would agree with me, I
suspect, about the specialness of her own dog. Reason argues that
everyone must be wrong: by definition, not every dog can be the
special dog—else special becomes ordinary. But it is reason that is
wrong: what is special is the life story that each dog owner
creates with and knows about his own dog. I am not exempt from
feeling that, even from a scientific vantage. Behavioral scientific
approaches to dogs, far from displacing this story, simply build on
the singular understanding of the dog owner—on the expertise that
each dog owner has about her dog.
When Pump was nearly at the end of her life,
undeniably old, she lost weight, her muzzle grayed, she slowed
sometimes to a stop on walks. I saw her frustrations, her
resignations, her impulses pursued or abandoned; I saw her
considerations, her control, her calm. But when I looked at her
face, and into her eyes, she was a puppy again. I saw glimpses of
that unnamed dog who so cooperatively let us plop a too-big collar
around her neck and walk her out of the shelter and thirty blocks
home. And then thousands of miles since.
After knowing Pump, and losing Pump, I met
Finnegan. I already cannot imagine not knowing this new character:
this leaner on legs, this stealer of balls, this warmer of laps. He
is incredibly unlike Pumpernickel. Yet what she taught me has made
every moment with Finnegan infinitely richer.
She lifted her head and turned toward me, her head pulsing slightly with her breathing. Her nose was dark and wet, her eyes calm. She began licking, full long licks of her front legs, of the floor. The tags of her collar clonked on the wood. Her ears lay flat, curling up a little at the bottom like a felted leaf, dried in the sun. Those days her front toes were a little splayed, her paws turned clawlike as though in preparation to pounce. She did not pounce. She yawned. It was a long, lazy afternoon yawn, her tongue languidly examining the air. She settled her head down between her legs, exhaled a kind of har-ummmp, and closed her eyes.
Notes and Sources
In addition to the sources listed by chapter
below, I refer to the following books frequently. Each is a
scholarly yet accessible approach to dog behavior, cognition, or
training; I recommend them all for anyone interested in further
details of dog science.
Lindsay, S. R. 2000, 2001, 2005. Handbook of applied dog behavior and training (3
volumes). Ames, Iowa: Blackwell Publishing.
McGreevy, P., and R. A. Boakes. 2007.
Carrots and sticks: Principles of
animal training. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Miklósi, Á. 2007. Dog
behavior, evolution, and cognition. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Serpell, J., ed. 1995. The domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour and
interactions with people.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
PRELUDE
on determining species
brain differences:
Rogers, L. 2004. Increasing the brain's
capacity: Neocortex, new neurons, and hemispheric specialization.
In L. J. Rogers, and G. Kaplan, eds. Comparative vertebrate
cognition: Are primates superior to non-primates? (pp.
289–324). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
UMWELT
on the dolphin
smile:
Bearzi, M., and C. B. Stanford. 2008.
Beautiful minds: The parallel lives of
Great Apes and dolphins.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
on the fear grin in
chimpanzees:
Chadwick-Jones, J. 2000. Developing a social psychology of monkeys and apes.
East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.
on eyebrow-raising in monkeys:
Kyes, R. C., and D. K. Candland. 1987. Baboon
(Papio hamadryas) visual preferences
for regions of the face. Journal of
Comparative Psychology, 4, 345–348.
de Waal, F. B. M., M. Dindo, C. A. Freeman,
and M. J. Hall. 2005. The monkey in the mirror: Hardly a stranger.
Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 102,
11140–11147.
on chicken
preferences:
Febrer, K., T. A. Jones, C. A. Donnelly, and
M. S. Dawkins. 2006. Forced to crowd or choosing to cluster?
Spatial distribution indicates social attraction in broiler
chickens. Animal Behaviour, 72,
1291–1300.
on muzzle biting and
standing-over in wolves:
Fox, M. W. 1971. Behaviour of wolves, dogs and related canids. New
York: Harper & Row.
on shock
experiments:
Seligman, M. E. P., S. F. Maier, and J. H.
Geer. 1965. Alleviation of learned helplessness in the dog.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 73,
256–262.
on umwelt, ticks, and
functional tones:
von Uexküll, J. 1957/1934. A stroll through
the worlds of animals and men. In C. H. Schiller, ed. Instinctive behavior: The development of a modern
concept (pp. 5–80). New York: International Universities
Press.
on pessimistic
rats:
Harding, E. J., E. S. Paul, and M. Mendl.
2004. Cognitive bias and affective state. Nature, 427, 312.
on dog
kisses:
Fox, 1971.
on the dog's sense of
taste:
Lindemann, B. 1996. Taste reception.
Physiological Reviews, 76, 719–766.
Serpell, 1995.
"dogs have … a striking
way of exhibiting their affection …"
Darwin, C. 1872/1965. The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, p. 118.
BELONGING TO THE HOUSE
on the variety of
canids:
Macdonald, D. W., and C. Sillero-Zubiri. 2004.
The biology and conservation of
wild canids. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
on raisin
toxicity:
McKnight, K. Feb. 2005. Toxicology brief:
Grape and raisin toxicity in dogs. Veterinary
Technician, 26, 135–136.
etymology of
"domesticated":
I drew this wording from Samuel Johnson's 1755
dictionary: domestical and domestick are both partially defined "belonging to
the house; not relating to things publick."
on the fox domestication
experiments:
Belyaev, D. K. 1979. Destabilizing selection
as a factor in domestication. Journal
of Heredity, 70,
301–308.
Trut, L. N. 1999. Early canid domestication:
The farm-fox experiment. American
Scientist, 87, 160–169.
on wolf behavior and
anatomy:
Mech, D. L., and L. Boitani. 2003.
Wolves: Behavior, ecology, and
conservation. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
on
domestication:
There are many current theories of dog
domestication. The one presented here is corroborated by both the
recent mtDNA findings, and by a better understanding of the
genetics of selection. It is elaborated in R. Coppinger and L.
Coppinger. 2001. Dogs: A startling new
understanding of canine origin, behavior, and evolution. New York: Scribner.
Clutton-Brock, J. 1999. A
natural history of domesticated mammals, 2nd ed. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
on earliest date of
domestication:
Ostrander, E. A., U. Giger, and K. Lindblad-Toh, eds. 2006. The dog and its genome. Cold Spring Harbor, NY: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press.
Vilà, C., P. Savolainen, J. E. Maldonado, I.
R. Amorim, J. E. Rice, R. L. Honeycutt, K. A. Crandall, J.
Lundeberg, and R. K. Wayne. 1997. Multiple and ancient origins of
the domestic dog. Science, 276,
1687–1689.
on
development:
Mech and Boitani, 2003.
Scott, J. P., and J. L. Fuller. 1965.
Genetics and the social behaviour of the
dog. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
on poodle/husky
difference in development:
Feddersen-Petersen, D., in Miklósi,
2007.
on wolf rope
task:
Miklósi, Á., E. Kubinyi, J. Topál, M. Gácsi,
Zs. Virányi, and V. Csányi. 2003. A simple reason for a big
difference: Wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do.
Current Biology, 13, 763–766.
on eye
contact:
Fox, 1971.
Serpell, J. 1996. In the
company of animals: A study of human-animal relationships.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
on
breeds:
Garber, M. 1996. Dog
love. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Ostrander et al., 2006.
on leg length-chest depth
ratios:
Brown, C. M. 1986. Dog
locomotion and gait analysis. Wheat Ridge, CO: Hoflin
Publishing Ltd.
on the Ibizan and
Pharaoh:
Parker, H. G, L. V. Kim, N. B. Sutter, S.
Carlson, T. D. Lorentzen, T. B. Malek, G. S. Johnson, H. B.
DeFrance, E. A. Ostrander, and L. Kruglyak. 2004. Genetic structure
of the purebred domestic dog. Science,
304, 1160–1164.
on breed
specs:
Crowley, J., and B. Adelman, eds. 1998. The complete dog book, 19th edition. Publication of the American Kennel Club. New York: Howell Book House.
on the dog
genome:
Kirkness, E. F., et
al. 2003. The dog genome: Survey sequencing and comparative
analysis. Science, 301,
1898–1903.
Lindblad-Toh, K., et al. 2005. Genome
sequence, comparative analysis and haplotype structure of the
domestic dog. Nature, 438,
803–819.
Ostrander et al., 2006.
Parker et al., 2004.
on aggressive
breeds:
Duffy, D. L., Y. Hsu, and J. A. Serpell. 2008.
Breed differences in canine aggression. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 114,
441–460.
on sheepdog
behavior:
Coppinger and Coppinger, 2001.
on
packs:
Mech, L. D. 1999. Alpha status, dominance, and
division of labor in wolf packs. Canadian
Journal of Zoology, 77, 1196–1203.
Mech and Boitani, 2003, especially L. D. Mech,
and L. Boitani. "Wolf social ecology" (pp. 1–34) and Packard, J. M.
"Wolf behavior: Reproductive, social, and intelligent" (pp.
35–65).
on dog and wolf
tracks:
Miller, D. 1981. Track
Finder. Rochester, NY: Nature Study Guild
Publishers.
on feral
dogs:
Beck, A. M. 2002. The
ecology of stray dogs: A study of free-ranging urban
animals. West Lafayette, IN: NotaBell
Books.
on Italian free-ranging
dogs:
Cafazzo, S., P. Valsecchi, C. Fantini, and E.
Natoli. 2008. Social dynamics of a group of free-ranging domestic
dogs living in a suburban environment. Paper presented at Canine
Science Forum, Budapest, Hungary.
on the wolf socialization
project:
Kubinyi, E., Zs. Virányi, and Á Miklósi. 2007. Comparative social cognition: From wolf and dog to humans. Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 2, 26–46. "blooming, buzzing confusion":
William James used these words to describe the
lack of organization of the information that an infant first
receives through her inchoate senses: James, W. 1890. Principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt
& Co., p. 488.
"white and shapeless lump
of flesh …":
Pliny the Elder. Natural
history (tr. H. Rackham, 1963), Volume 3. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press, Book 8(54).
SNIFF
generally interesting
readings on smell:
Drobnick, J., ed. 2006. The smell culture reader. New York: Berg.
Sacks, O. 1990. "The dog beneath the skin." In
The man who mistook his wife for a
hat and other clinical tales (pp.
156–160). New York: HarperPerennial.
on
sniffing:
Settles, G. S., D. A. Kester, and L. J.
Dodson-Dreibelbis. 2003. The external aerodynamics of canine
olfaction. In F. G. Barth, J. A. C. Humphrey, and T. W. Secomb,
eds. Sensors and sensing in biology and
engineering (pp. 323–355). New York: SpringerWein.
on the anatomy and
sensitivity of the nose:
Harrington, F. H., and C. S. Asa. 2003. Wolf
communication. In D. Mech, and L. Boitani, eds. Wolves: Behavior, ecology and conservation (pp.
66–103). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lindsay, 2000.
Serpell, 1995.
Wright, R. H. 1982. The
sense of smell. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
on the vomeronasal
organ:
Adams, D. R., and M. D. Wiekamp. 1984. The
canine vomeronasal organ. Journal
of Anatomy, 138, 771–787.
Sommerville, B. A., and D. M. Broom. 1998.
Olfactory awareness. Applied Animal
Behavior Science, 57,
269–286.
Watson, L. 2000. Jacobson's organ and the remarkable nature of smell. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
on human pheromone
detection:
Jacob, S., and M. K. McClintock. 2000.
Psychological state and mood effects of steroidal chemosignals in
women and men. Hormones and Behavior,
37, 57–78.
McClintock, M. K. 1971. Menstrual synchrony
and suppression. Nature, 229,
244–245.
on moist
noses:
Mason, R. T., M. P. LeMaster, and D.
Muller-Schwarze. 2005. Chemical signals
in vertebrates, Volume 10. New
York: Springer.
on smelling
us:
Lindsay, 2000.
on distinguishing twins
by scent:
Hepper, P. G. 1988. The discrimination of
human odor by the dog. Perception, 17,
549–554.
on bloodhounds:
Lindsay, 2000.
Sommerville and Broom, 1998. Watson,
2000.
on using footsteps to
detect trail:
Hepper, P. G, and D. L. Wells. 2005. How many
footsteps do dogs need to determine the direction of an odour
trail? Chemical Senses, 30,
291–298.
Syrotuck, W. G. 1972. Scent and the scenting dog. Mechanicsburg, PA:
Barkleigh Productions.
on the smell of
tuberculosis:
Wright, 1982.
on the smell of
disease:
Drobnick, 2006. Syrotuck, 1972.
on cancer
detection:
a partial list of the many studies:
McCulloch, M., T. Jezierski, M. Broffman, A.
Hubbard, K. Turner, and T. Janecki. 2006. Diagnostic accuracy of
canine scent detection in early-and late-stage lung and breast
cancers. Integrative Cancer Therapies,
5,30–39.
Williams, H., and A. Pembroke. 1989. Sniffer
dogs in the melanoma clinic? Lancet,
1, 734.
Willis, C. M., S. M. Church, C. M. Guest, W.
A. Cook, N. McCarthy, A. J. Bransbury, M. R. T. Church, and J. C.
T. Church. 2004. Olfactory detection of bladder cancer by dogs:
Proof of principle study. British Medical
Journal, 329, 712–716.
on epileptic seizure
detection:
Dalziel, D. J., B. M. Uthman, S. P. McGorray,
and R. L. Reep. 2003. Seizure-alert dogs: A review and preliminary
study. Seizure, 12, 115–120.
Doherty, M. J., and A. M. Haltiner. 2007. Wag
the dog: Skepticism on seizure alert canines. Neurology, 68, 309.
Kirton, A., E. Wirrell, J. Zhang, and L.
Hamiwka. 2004. Seizure-alerting and -response behaviors in dogs
living with epileptic children. Neurology,
62, 2303–2305.
on urine
marking:
Lindsay, 2005.
Lorenz, K. 1954. Man
meets dog. London: Methuen.
on bladders' single
use:
Sapolsky, R. M. 2004. Why
zebras don't get ulcers. New York: Henry Holt &
Company.
on anal
sacs:
Harrington and Asa, 2003.
Natynczuk, S., J. W. S. Bradshaw, and D. W.
Macdonald. 1989. Chemical constituents of the anal sacs of domestic
dogs. Biochemical Systematics and
Ecology, 17, 83–87.
on anal sacs and
vets:
McGreevy, P. (personal
communication).
on scratching the ground
after marking:
Bekoff, M. 1979. Ground scratching by male domestic dogs: A composite signal. Journal of Mammalogy, 60, 847–848.
on antibiotics and
smell:
Attributed to John Bradshaw by Coghlan, A.
September 23, 2006. Animal welfare: See things from their
perspective. NewScientist.com.
on the Manhattan
grid:
Margolies, E. 2006. Vagueness gridlocked: A
map of the smells of New York. In J. Drobnick, ed., The smell culture reader (pp. 107–117). New York:
Berg.
on brambish and
brunky:
These words were coined by Calvin and Hobbes artist Bill Watterson, and put in
the mouth of his cartoon tiger Hobbes.
"brilliant smell of water
…":
Chesterton, G. K. 2004. "The song of the
quoodle," in The collected works of G.
K. Chesterton. San Francisco:
Ignatius Press, p. 556. (In this same poem he commented on the
relative "noselessness" of man.)
MUTE
"blank
bewilderment":
Woolf, V. 1933. Flush: A
biography. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, p.
44.
"uncommunicating
muteness":
Lamb, C. 1915. Essays of
Elia. London: J. M. Dent & Sons, Ltd., p. 53.
on the dog's hearing
range:
Harrington and Asa, 2003.
on the "Mosquito"
teenager repellent:
Vitello, P. June 12, 2006. "A ring tone meant
to fall on deaf ears." The New York
Times.
on alarm
clocks:
Bodanis, D. 1986. The
secret house: 24 hours in the strange and unexpected world
in which we spend our nights and days.
New York: Simon & Schuster.
on growls:
Faragó, T., F. Range, Zs. Virányi, and P.
Pongrácz. 2008. The bone is mine! Context-specific vocalisation in
dogs. Paper presented at Canine Science Forum, Budapest,
Hungary.
on dog and wolf
sounds:
Fox, 1971.
Harrington and Asa, 2003.
on
laughs:
Simonet, O., M. Murphy, and A. Lance. 2001.
Laughing dog: Vocalizations of domestic dogs during play
encounters. Animal Behavior Society conference, Corvallis,
OR.
on distinguishing
high-pitched sounds:
McConnell, P. B. 1990. Acoustic structure and
receiver response in domestic dogs, Canis
familiaris. Animal Behaviour, 39, 897–904.
on Rico and other
vocabularians:
Kaminski, J. 2008. Dogs' understanding of
human forms of communication. Paper presented at the Canine Science
Forum, Budapest, Hungary.
Kaminski, J., J. Call, and J. Fischer. 2004.
Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for "fast mapping."
Science, 304, 1682–1683.
on conversational
maxims:
Grice, P. 1975. Logic and conversation. In P.
Cole and J. L. Morgan, eds., Speech
acts (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic
Press.
on whimpers, barks, and
other vocalizations:
Bradshaw, J. W. S., and H. M. R. Nott. 1995.
Social and communication behaviour of companion dogs. In J.
Serpell, ed., The domestic dog: Its evolution,
behaviour, and interactions with
people (pp. 115–130). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Cohen, J. A., and M. W. Fox. 1976.
Vocalizations in wild canids and possible effects of domestication.
Behavioural Processes, 1,
77–92.
Harrington and Asa, 2003.
Tembrock, G. 1976. Canid vocalizations.
Behavioural Processes, 1,
57–75.
on characteristics and kinds of barks:
Molnár, C., P. Pongrácz, A. Dóka, and Á.
Miklósi. 2006. Can humans discriminate between dogs on the base of
the acoustic parameters of barks? Behavioural Processes,
73, 76–83.
Yin, S., and B. McCowan. 2004. Barking in domestic dogs: Context specificity and individual identification. Animal Behaviour, 68, 343–355.
on dog bark
decibels:
Moffat et al.
2003. Effectiveness and comparison of citronella and scentless
spray bark collars for the control of barking in a veterinary
hospital setting. Journal of
the American Animal Hospital Association,
39, 343–348.
"But man himself cannot
express love and humility …":
Darwin, C. 1872/1965, p. 10.
on
hackles:
Harrington and Asa, 2003.
on
antithesis:
Darwin, 1872/1965.
on
tails:
Bradshaw and Nott, 1995.
Harrington and Asa, 2003.
Schenkel, R. 1947. Expression studies of
wolves. Behaviour, 1, 81–129.
on
posture:
Fox, 1971.
Goodwin, D., J. W. S. Bradshaw, and S. M.
Wickens. 1997. Paedomorphosis affects agonistic visual signals of
domestic dogs. Animal Behaviour, 53,
297–304.
on intentional
communication:
Kaminski, J. 2008.
more on urine
marking:
Bekoff, M. 1979. Scent-marking by free ranging
domestic dogs. Olfactory and visual components. Biology of Behaviour, 4, 123–139.
Bradshaw and Nott, 1995.
Pal, S. K. 2003. Urine marking by free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris) in relation to sex, season, place and posture. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 80, 45–59.
DOG-EYED
on the visual range of canids:
Harrington and Asa, 2003. Miklósi, 2007.
on distribution of
photoreceptors in retinae:
McGreevy, P., T. D. Grassia, and A. M.
Harmanb. 2004. A strong correlation exists between the distribution
of retinal ganglion cells and nose length in the dog. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 63, 13–22.
Neitz, J., T. Geist, and G. H. Jacobs. 1989.
Color vision in the dog. Visual
Neuroscience, 3, 119–25.
on arctic
wolves:
Packard, J. 2008. Man meets wolf: Ethological
perspectives. Paper presented at Canine Science Forum, Budapest,
Hungary.
on
Frisbee-catching:
Shaffer, D. M., S. M. Krauchunas, M. Eddy, and
M. K. McBeath. 2004. How dogs navigate to catch frisbees.
Psychological Science, 15,
437–441.
on dogs' recognition of
their owners' faces:
Adachi, I., H. Kuwahata, and K. Fujita. 2007.
Dogs recall their owner's face upon hearing the owner's voice.
Animal Cognition, 10, 17–21.
on cows noticing visual
details:
Grandin, T., and C. Johnson. 2006.
Animals in translation: Using the mysteries
of autism to decode animal
behavior. Orlando, FL: Harcourt.
SEEN BY A DOG
on imprinting in
geese:
Lorenz, K. 1981. The
foundations of ethology. New York:
Springer-Verlag.
on newborn and infant humans' visual abilities and development:
The information about infants' visual
abilities comes from a century of research. A nice summary is given
in Smith, P. K., H. Cowie, and M. Blades. 2003. Understanding children's development. Malden, MA:
Blackwell Publishing.
on infant tongue
protrusion:
Meltzoff, A. N., and M. K. Moore. 1977.
Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates.
Science, 198, 75–78. (They not only
stuck out their tongues at day-or even hour-old infants. They also
pursed their lips and opened their mouths wide as if in surprise.
Even newborns repeated these expressions back at them—or tried to:
lip-pursing is probably not a motor ability voluntarily available
to the newly born.)
on
Kanzi:
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., and R. Lewin. 1996.
Kanzi: The ape at the brink of the
human mind. New York: John Wiley &
Sons.
on Alex:
Pepperberg, I. M. 1999. The Alex studies: Cognitive and communicative abilities
of grey parrots. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
on the
dog-keyboard:
Rossi, A., and C. Ades. 2008. A dog at the
keyboard: Using arbitrary signs to communicate requests.
Animal Cognition, 11,
329–338.
on gaze
avoidance:
Bradshaw and Nott, 1995.
on dogs looking at
faces:
Miklósi et al., 2003.
on breeders preferring
dark eyes:
Serpell, 1996.
on gull fixed action
pattern:
Tinbergen, N. 1953. The
herring-gull's world. London: Collins.
on gaze in human
conversation:
Argyle, M., and J. Dean. 1965. Eye contact, distance and affiliation. Sociometry, 28, 289–304.
Vertegaal, R., R. Slagter, G. C. Van der Veer,
and A. Nijholt. 2001. Eye gaze patterns in conversations: There is
more to conversational agents than meets the eyes. In Proceedings of ACM CHI 2001 Conference on Human Factors
in Computing Systems, Seattle,
WA.
on following a pointing
gesture:
Soproni, K., Á. Miklósi, J. Topál, and V.
Csányi. 2002. Dogs' responsiveness to human pointing gestures.
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116,
27–34.
on
gaze-following:
Agnetta, B., B. Hare, and M. Tomasello. 2000.
Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not
use. Animal Cognition, 3,
107–112.
on
attention-getting:
Horowitz, A. 2009. Attention to attention in
domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic
play. Animal Cognition, 12,
107–118.
on sonorous
mouth-licking:
Gaunet, F. 2008. How do guide dogs of blind
owners and pet dogs of sighted owners (Canis
familiaris) ask their owners for food? Animal Cognition, 11, 475–483.
on
showing:
Hare, B., J. Call, and M. Tomasello. 1998.
Communication of food location between human and dog (Canis familiaris). Evolution
of Communication, 2, 137–159.
Miklósi, Á., R. Polgardi, J. Topál, and V.
Csányi. 2000. Intentional behaviour in dog-human communication: An
experimental analysis of "showing" behaviour in the dog.
Animal Cognition, 3, 159–166.
on retrieving
games:
Gácsi, M., Á. Miklósi, O. Varga, J. Topál, and
V. Csányi. 2004. Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs
(Canis familiaris) show
situation-dependent recognition of human's attention. Animal Cognition, 7, 144–153.
on manipulating
attention:
Call, J., J. Brauer, J. Kaminski, and M. Tomasello. 2003. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 257–263.
Schwab, C., and L. Huber. 2006. Obey or not
obey? Dogs (Canis familiaris) behave
differently in response to attentional states of their owners.
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 169–175.
on begging
experiments:
Cooper, J. J., C. Ashton, S. Bishop, R. West,
D. S. Mills, and R. J. Young. 2003. Clever hounds: Social cognition
in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris).
Applied Animal
Behaviour Science, 81, 229–244.
on attending to a video
projection:
Pongrácz, P., Á. Miklósi, A. Doka, and V.
Csányi. 2003. Successful application of video-projected human
images for signalling to dogs. Ethology,
109, 809–821.
on why commands relayed
by speakers don't work:
Virányi, Zs., J. Topál, M. Gácsi, Á. Miklósi,
and V. Csányi. 2004. Dogs can recognize the behavioural cues of the
attentional focus in humans. Behavioural Processes,
66, 161–172.
CANINE ANTHROPOLOGISTS
"I am I
…":
Stein, G. 1937. Everybody's Autobiography. New York: Random House,
p. 64.
on autistic people using
dogs to read others:
Sacks, O. 1995. An
anthropologist on Mars. New York: Knopf.
on Clever
Hans:
Sebeok, T. A., and R. Rosenthal, eds. 1981.
The Clever Hans phenomenon:
Communication with horses, whales, apes, and
people. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
on dogs reading trainers'
body movements:
Wright, 1982.
on dogs anticipating us
on walks:
Kubinyi, E., Á. Miklósi, J. Topál, and V.
Csányi. 2003. Social mimetic behaviour and social anticipation in
dogs: Preliminary results. Animal Cognition,
6, 57–63.
on distinguishing threatening and friendly strangers:
Vas, J., J. Topál, M. Gácsi, Á. Miklósi, and
V. Csányi. 2005. A friend or an enemy? Dogs' reaction to an
unfamiliar person showing behavioural cues of threat and
friendliness at different times. Applied
Animal Behaviour Science, 94, 99–115.
NOBLE MIND
on
neophilia:
Kaulfuss, P., and D. S. Mills. 2008. Neophilia
in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and
its implication for studies of dog cognition. Animal Cognition, 11, 553–556.
on physical
cognition:
Miklósi, 2007.
on
string-pulling:
Osthaus, B., S. E. G. Lea, and A. M. Slater.
2005. Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris)
fail to show understanding of means-end connections in a
string-pulling task. Animal
Cognition, 8, 37–47.
on use of social
cues:
Erdohegyi, A., J. Topál, Zs. Virányi, and Á.
Miklósi. 2007. Dog-logic: Inferential reasoning in a two-way choice
task and its restricted use. Animal Behavior,
74, 725–737.
on dogs looking to humans
to solve task:
Miklósi et al., 2003.
on milk-bottle
tits:
Fisher, J., and R. A. Hinde. 1949. The opening
of milk bottles by birds. British
Birds, 42, 347–357.
on chickadees
experiment:
Sherry, D. F., and B. G. Galef Jr. 1990.
Social learning without imitation: More about milk bottle opening
by birds. Animal Behaviour, 40,
987–989.
on detour
learning:
Pongrácz, P., Á. Miklósi, K. Timar-Geng, and
V. Csányi. 2004. Verbal attention getting as a key factor in social
learning between dog (Canis familiaris)
and human. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
118, 375–383.
on infant imitation:
Gergely, G., H. Bekkering, and I. Király.
2002. Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.
Whiten, A., D. M. Custance, J-C. Gomez, P.
Teixidor, and K. A. Bard. 1996. Imitative learning of artificial
fruit processing in children (Homo
sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan
troglodytes). Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 110, 3–14.
on dog
imitation:
Range, F., Zs. Virányi, and L. Huber. 2007.
Selective imitation in domestic dogs. Current
Biology, 17, 868–872.
on "do it"
task:
Topál, J., R. W. Byrne, Á. Miklósi, and V.
Csányi. 2006. Reproducing human actions and action sequences: "Do
as I Do!" in a dog. Animal Cognition,
9, 355–367.
on theory of
mind:
Premack, D., and G. Woodruff. 1978. Does a
chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.
on false belief
test:
Wimmer, H., and J. Perner. 1983. Beliefs about
beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs
in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.
on Philip, the dog who
informs about the keys:
Topál, J., A. Erdõhegyi, R. Mányik, and Á.
Miklósi. 2006. Mindreading in a dog: An adaptation of a primate
"mental attribution" study. International
Journal of Psychology and Psychological
Therapy, 6, 365–379.
on the function of
play:
Bekoff, M., and J. Byers, eds. 1998.
Animal play: Evolutionary, comparative,
and ecological perspectives.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fagen, R. 1981. Animal
play behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
on play-fighting not
improving later fighting skills:
Martin, P., and T. M. Caro. 1985. On the
functions of play and its role in behavioral development.
Advances in the Study of Behavior, 15,
59–103.
more on dogs' use of attention, attention-getters, and communication in play:
Horowitz, 2009.
on play
signals:
Bekoff, M. 1972. The development of social
interaction, play, and meta-communication in mammals: An
ethological perspective. Quarterly
Review of Biology, 47,
412–434.
Bekoff, M. 1995. Play signals as punctuation:
The structure of social play in canids. Behaviour, 132, 419–429.
Horowitz, 2009.
on the (un)fairness
experiment:
Range, F., L. Horn, Zs. Virányi, and L. Huber.
2009. The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs.
Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 106, 340–345.
INSIDE OF A DOG
on
counting:
West, R. E., and R. J. Young. 2002. Do
domestic dogs show any evidence of being able to count?
Animal Cognition, 5, 183–186.
on disjunctive
syllogisms:
This is the stoic philosopher Chrysippos of
Soloi, according to Bringmann, W., and J. Abresch. 1997. Clever
Hans: Fact or fiction? In W. G. Bringmann et al., eds.,
A pictorial
history of psychology (pp. 77–82). Chicago:
Quintessence.
one of the original
scientific attempts to try to operationalize
anthropomorphisms:
Hebb, D. O. 1946. Emotion in man and animal:
An analysis of the intuitive process of recognition. Psychological Review, 53, 88–106.
on the suprachiasmatic
nucleus:
A nice review of some recent work: Herzog, E.
D., and L. J. Muglia. 2006. You are when you eat. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 300–302.
on changes in sleep with
age:
Takeuchi, T., and E. Harada. 2002. Age-related
changes in sleep-wake rhythm in dog. Behavioural Brain Research, 136, 193–199.
on the movement of smells in a room:
Bodanis, 1986. Wright, 1982.
on bees' sense of
time:
Boisvert, M. J., and D. F. Sherry. 2006.
Interval timing by an invertebrate, the bumble bee Bombus impatiens. Current Biology, 16,
1636–1640.
"boredom is rarely
discussed in the non-human scientific literature":
But see Wemelsfelder, F. 2005. Animal Boredom:
Understanding the tedium of confined lives. In F. D. McMillan, ed.,
Mental health and well-being in animals
(pp. 79–91). Ames, Iowa: Blackwell Publishing.
"man is the only animal
who can be bored":
Fromm, E. 1947. Man for
himself, an inquiry into the psychology of ethics. New York:
Rinehart, p. 40.
on the mirror
test:
Gallup, G. G. Jr. 1970. Chimpanzees:
Self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–87.
Plotnik, J. M., F. B. M. de Waal, and D. Reiss. 2006.
Self-recognition in an
Asian elephant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science,
103,17053–17057.
Reiss, D., and L. Marino. 2001. Mirror
self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive
convergence. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Science, 98,
5937–3942.
on sheepdogs' knowing
they are not sheep:
Coppinger and Coppinger, 2001.
Snoopy
quote:
Gesner, C. 1967. You're a
good man, Charlie Brown: Based on the comic strip Peanuts
by Charles M. Schulz. New York: Random
House.
on scrub-jay
caching:
Raby, C. R., D. M. Alexis, A. Dickinson, and
N. S. Clayton. 2007. Planning for the future by western scrub-jays.
Nature, 445, 919–921.
on ontogenetic
ritualization:
Tomasello, M., and J. Call. 1997. Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.
on medieval punishment of
dogs:
Evans, E. P. 1906/2000. The criminal prosecution and capital punishment of
animals. Union, NJ: Lawbook Exchange,
Ltd.
on owners thinking dogs
know right and wrong:
Pongrácz, P., Á. Miklósi, and V. Csányi. 2001.
Owners' beliefs on the ability of their pet dogs to understand
human verbal communication: A case of social understanding.
Cahiers de psychologie, 20,
87–107.
on teddy-bear
guard-dog:
Kennedy, M. August 3, 2006. "Guard dog mauls
Elvis's teddy in rampage." The
Guardian.
on guilt
experiments:
Horowitz, A. 2009. Disambiguating the "guilty
look": Salient prompts to a familiar dog behaviour. Behavioural Processes, 81, 447–452.
Vollmer, P. J. 1977. Do mischievous dogs
reveal their "guilt"? Veterinary
Medicine, Small Animal Clinician, 72,
1002–1005.
on the blind Labrador
Norman:
Goodall, J., and M. Bekoff. 2002. The ten trusts: What we must do to care for the
animals we love. New York:
HarperCollins.
on emergency
experiment:
Macpherson, K., and W. A. Roberts. 2006. Do
dogs (Canis familiaris) seek help in an
emergency? Journal of Comparative Psychology,
120, 113–119.
"What is it like to be a
bat?":
Nagel, T. 1974. What is it like to be a bat?
Philosophical Review, 83,
435–450.
on Stanley's view of the
world:
Sterbak, J. 2003. "From here to
there."
on personal
space:
Argyle and Dean, 1965.
on differences in heeling
styles:
Packard, 2008.
on a snail's perception
of a tapping stick:
von Uexküll, 1957/1934.
on pressure release as
reinforcement in horses:
McGreevy and Boakes, 2007.
on slaughterhouse
design:
Grandin and Johnson, 2005.
on perception of objects
under yellow light:
I owe my understanding of the blood-draining
effect of yellow light to the exhibit "Room for one colour" by the
artist Olafur Eliasson, in which he lights a room by bulbs emitting
an extremely narrow range of what appears as yellow
light.
Wittgenstein on
dogs:
Wittgenstein, L. 1953. Philosophical investigations. New York:
Macmillan.
on the length of a
moment:
von Uexküll, 1957/1934.
on clicker
training:
McGreevy and Boakes, 2007.
on the wolves'
provocative showing of food:
Miklósi, 2007.
YOU HAD ME AT HELLO
on vasopressin in the
prairie vole:
Alcock, J. 2005. Animal
behavior: An evolutionary approach, 8th ed. Sunder-land, MA:
Sinauer Associates.
on sheepdog
imprinting:
Coppinger and Coppinger, 2001.
on not all animals being equally anthropomorphizable:
Eddy, T. J., G. G. Gallup Jr., and D. J.
Povinelli. 1993. Attribution of cognitive states to animals:
Anthropomorphism in comparative perspective. Journal of Social Issues,
49, 87–101.
on our attraction to
infants and other neotonized creatures:
Gould, S. J. 1979. Mickey Mouse meets Konrad
Lorenz. Natural History, 88,
30–36.
Lorenz, K. 1950/1971. Ganzheit und Teil in der
tierischen und menschlichen Gemeinschaft. Reprinted in R. Martin,
ed., Studies in animal and human
behaviour, vol. 2 (pp. 115–195).
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
"we need the
eggs":
Said by Woody Allen's alter ego Alvy Singer in
Annie Hall, 1977.
on
biophilia:
Wilson, E. O. 1984. Biophilia. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
on
touch:
Lindsay, 2000.
on Harlow
studies:
Harlow, H. F. 1958. The nature of love.
American Psychologist, 13,
673–685.
Harlow, H. F., and S. J. Suomii. 1971. Social
recovery by isolation-reared monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
68, 1534–1538.
on the alleviation of
puppies' distress with soft toys:
Elliot, O., and J. P. Scott. 1961. The
development of emotional distress reactions to separation in
puppies. Journal of Genetic Psychology,
99, 3–22.
Pettijohn, T. F., T. W. Wong, P. D. Ebert, and
J. P. Scott. 1977. Alleviation of separation distress in 3 breeds
of young dogs. Developmental Psychobiology,
10, 373–381.
"thermotactile sensory
probe":
Fox, M. 1971. Socio-infantile and socio-sexual
signals in canids: A comparative and developmental study.
Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie, 28,
185–210.
on our tactile
resolution:
Attributed to the psychophysicist Ernst Heinrich Weber by von Uexküll (1957/ 1934).
on
whiskers:
Lindsay, 2000.
"redirected appeasement
ceremony":
Lorenz, K. 1966. On
aggression. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, Inc., p.
170.
on guide dogs and the
blind:
Naderi, Sz., Á. Miklósi, A. Dóka, and V.
Csányi. 2001. Cooperative interactions between blind persons and
their dog. Applied Animal Behavior Sciences,
74, 59–80.
on dog-human
play:
Horowitz, A. C., and M. Bekoff. 2007.
Naturalizing anthropomorphism: Behavioral prompts to our humanizing
of animals. Anthrozoös, 20,
23–35.
on timing patterns of
flirters:
Sakaguchi, K., G. K. Jonsson, and T. Hasegawa.
2005. Initial interpersonal attraction between mixed-sex dyad and
movement synchrony. In L. Anolli, S. Duncan Jr., M. S. Magnusson,
and G. Riva, eds., The hidden structure
of interaction: From neurons to culture
patterns (pp. 107–120). Amsterdam: IOS Press.
on synchrony between dogs
and people:
Kerepesi, A., G. K. Jonsson, Á. Miklósi, V.
Csányi, and M. S. Magnusson. 2005. Detection of temporal patterns
in dog–human interaction. Behavioural
Processes, 70, 69–79.
on dogs' sensitivity to
cortisol and testosterone:
Jones, A. C., and R. A. Josephs. 2006.
Interspecies hormonal interactions between man and the domestic dog
(Canis familiaris). Hormones and Behavior, 50, 393–400.
on dogs' sensitivity to
play styles:
Horváth, Zs., A. Dóka, and Á. Miklósi. 2008.
Affiliative and disciplinary behavior of human handlers during play
with their dog affects cortisol concentrations in opposite
directions. Hormones and Behavior, 54,
107–114.
on lowered blood pressure, other measures, and hormone changes:
Friedmann, E. 1995. The role of pets in
enhancing human well-being: Physiological effects. In I. Robinson,
ed., The Waltham book of human-animal
interactions: Benefits and
responsibilities of pet ownership (pp. 35–59). Oxford:
Pergamon.
Odendaal, J. S. J. 2000. Animal assisted
therapy—magic or medicine? Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 49,
275–280.
Wilson, C. C. 1991. The pet as an anxiolytic
intervention. Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 179, 482–489.
on other dog-owning
benefits:
Serpell, 1996.
on contagious
yawns:
Joly-Mascheroni, R. M., A. Senju, and A. J.
Shepherd. 2008. Dogs catch human yawns. Biology Letters, 4, 446–448.
on Derrida, naked, and
his cat:
Derrida, J. 2002. L'animal que donc je suis (à
suivre). Translated as "The animal that therefore I am (more to
follow)." Critical Inquiry, 28,
369–418.
THE IMPORTANCE OF MORNINGS
on herding and the
"eye":
Coppinger and Coppinger, 2001.
on handedness in
dogs:
P. McGreevy, personal communication.
on
training:
See McGreevy and Boakes, 2007, for some
ideas.
on preference for the
new:
Kaulfuss and Mills, 2008.
on dog
gaits:
Brown, 1986.
"colors one's thinking about it forever afterwards":
So said George Schaller, whose many books are
full of named animals. Quoted in Lehner, P. 1996. Handbook of ethological methods, 2nd ed. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press, p. 231.
on zebra-finch leg-band
preferences:
Burley, N. 1988. Wild zebra finches have band colour preferences. Animal Behaviour, 36, 1235–1237.
Acknowledgments
Of the following
dogs:
No one who knew Pumpernickel will be surprised
that my most ardent thanks go to her, for choosing us at the
shelter and for allowing me the incredible pleasure of knowing her.
I have thanked her many times since, with cheese taking over where
words failed me. Thanks to Finnegan, for being his own dog, and for
being such an utterly doggish dog. Every day is improved to have
him come running madly toward me. Thanks to the dogs of yore: to
Aster, who endured a lot of childhood foolishness and taught me how
to be less foolish; to Chester, who could grin and growl at the
same time; to Beckett and Heidi, who in death highlighted what is
precious; and to Barnaby, who in catness highlighted what is dog.
Of the following people:
One hears that books are difficult to write.
If so, this is not a book, for it was a delight to write, as it is
delightful to observe and be with dogs and think dog-thoughts
full-time. I was even more pleased that I would be handing the book
over to the people at Scribner, whom I could count on to make my
bagful of chapters into an actual book. I am indebted to Colin
Harrison for his tireless reading of drafts and for being open to
just about anything. Had I turned a book about dogs into one about
cats I suspect Colin would have acceded to it … as long as it was
still a good read. Many thanks to Susan Moldow for her enthusiasm
from the very beginning.
Before I had an agent I scanned
acknowledgments pages for those that included words that would send
me scurrying to shine up a proposal to their agents. Sorry, Kris
Dahl, in advance, for this: she is the very person you want
representing you and your book; and I thank her.
My graduate school advisors and mentors,
Shirley Strum and Jeff Elman, were willing to consider how an
abstruse theoretical question about cognition could be addressed by
observations of dogs—and they improved the theory and the practice.
I was and am still appreciative. Thanks to Aaron Cicourel, who is
also, as he says, one of those folks who try to saw through wood
the hard way. Marc Bekoff was one of the first to treat dog play as
biologically interesting. It was his writing (with the very keen
Colin Allen), and later his advice, devotion, and friendship that
led me to pursue my own research.
I owe thanks to Damon Horowitz, with whom I
hatched the plan to write this book, and who seemed to believe that
it was a sage and realistic idea. His consummate skepticism about
all matters is balanced by his unfettered support of all that
matters to me. I owe pretty much everything to my parents,
Elizabeth and Jay. They were the first people I wanted to show the
book to, for all the right reasons. As
for you, Ammon Shea: you make me better with words, you make me better with dogs, and you make me better.
Index
abstraction, 253–54
adaptation, 31–32, 70–71
addictive behavior, 53–54
adoption of dogs, 47, 65, 262n, 266–67, 294, 301
adrenaline, 80
affection, dog kisses and, 29–31
Afghan hound, 49, 127
age
in dog years, 222n
hearing decline with, 93n
knowledge of death, 235–37
in wolf hierarchies, 40, 58–59
aggression
body language showing, 109–10, 112
dachshunds and, 53n
designation in dogs, 52–55
in domestication process, 35
eye contact and, 147, 148–49
hormones and, 172
play versus, 5, 61–65, 270
sounds made and, 102
touch and, 293
of wolves, 59
agility training, 288
agonistic, 102
Aibo robot dog, 276
akinetopsia, 132
Alex (parrot), 145n
allelomimetic behavior, 274–79
Allen, Woody, 264–65
American Kennel Club, 49, 50
American Staffordshire terrier, 53n
anal sacs, 84–85, 112, 117
animal cognition, 4–7
anticipation in, 166–72
attention in, 144–59, 290–91
attitudes toward studying dogs, 3–4, 6–7
begging experiments, 155–57
big-brain hypothesis and, 8–9
learned optimism and pessimism, 27–28
play in, 196–205
primates as subjects of, 4–5, 102n
subjective experiences of dogs, 241–58
theory of mind and, 190–96
video cameras and, 5–6
what dogs know, 210–41
see also communication; physical
cognition; social cognition; training
animistic, 102n
anosmia, 72
anthropomorphism
adaptiveness of, 31–32
behavior-reading versus, 18–19, 26–28, 31–32
clothing for dogs, 17–19, 29, 56
history, 15
scientific attitude toward, 3–4
temperament and, 47–48
training and, 57–61
umwelt and, 14–17, 23, 31–32, 263n, 294–96
antibiotics, 85, 86
anticipating behavior, 166–72
antidepressants, 16
antithesis, 110, 112
area centralis, 127
artificial odors, 25–26, 69, 71, 72, 86, 292
artificial selection, 6, 35–37, 41
assistance dogs, 43, 134–35, 152, 162, 240, 274
associative learning (associations), 10–11, 167–68, 182, 225,
232–33, 289
attachment
of dogs, 42–43, 63–65
greeting and, 43, 271–72
of human infants, 43
of socialized wolves, 63–64
attacks, in rough-and-tumble play, 1–2, 24,
62, 196–98, 200, 202, 205–6, 221
attention, 139–59
of animals, 144–59, 290–91
avoiding, 143, 144
baited buckets and, 150–51
defined, 139
to dogs, 290–91
joint, 58
manipulating attention, 143, 155–58
to our behavior, 140–41
in play, 152–53, 199–205
"psychic powers" of animals, 163–72
to sounds around you, 251–52
stages in child development, 141–44
types of, 139, 142–44
see also attention-getter; eye
contact
attention-getter
bark, 98, 105–8, 152, 153, 154, 171
bump, 152
exaggerated retreat, 203
in-your-face, 152, 200, 203
kinds of, 152–53, 199–200
matching level of inattention, 203
pawing, 152
used in play, 152–53, 199–205
audition
auditory attention, 139
changes in, 169
importance of, 122
range of hearing in dogs, 92, 93–94
range of hearing in humans, 21–22, 23–24, 92–94
in rats, 28
ultrasonic sounds, 91, 93, 241–43
see also sounds made
Australian shepherd, 53
autism
lack of eye contact in, 140, 192
theory of mind and, 192
use of dogs to read behavior, 162
autobiographies, pet, 119, 225–27
baboons
animal cognition research on, 4
eyebrows raised in display, 16
baby talk, 95–96
ball dogs, 126–32, 154, 287
barks
as communication, 98, 105–8, 152, 153, 154, 171
contagious, 108
decibels of, 105
frequencies of, 105–6, 107–8
types of, 98–99, 100, 106–8
see also attention-getter; sounds
made
basenji, 50
basset hound, 77–78
bathing dogs, 85–86, 292
baths, 19, 86
bats
echolocation of, 91, 241–43
flehmen, 74
beagle, 49, 71, 72
bears, birth of, 64–65
beastomorphism, 60
beds
dog, 25–26, 86
human, 25–26
bees, 91, 215
begging
in dogs, 155–56
experimental trials using, 155–57
behavioral science
animal cognition research in, 4–5, 13–14,
27–28, 102n operant conditioning and, 10–11, 189–90
Bekoff, Marc, 237–38 Belgian Tervuren,
193–95
belly, exposing, 110
Belyaev, Dmitry, 35–37
big-brain hypothesis, 8–9, 44
bike-chasing, 170–71
biophilia hypothesis (Wilson), 265n
bird dogs, 43, 49, 54
biting
games based on, 42
meanings of, 62–63
muzzle biting, 18–19
bitter taste, 30 bladders, 60–61, 83, 115,
116
blind persons, dog companions for, 43, 134–35,
152, 162, 274
bloodhound, olfaction in tracking, 77–78
blue tit, milk-bottle raiding of, 185
see also imitation
body language, 91, 108–14, 118
bonding
components of, 266–79
cross-species, 259–82
dog-human, 259–82
of domesticated silver foxes, 37
greeting ritual and, 29, 30–31, 263–64, 271–73
impact of bond effect, 279–82
mutual responsiveness in, 263–66, 274–79
pair bond, 260–61
proximate/ultimate explanations for, 265–66
temporal patterns and, 274–79
touch and, 266–71, 293
bonobos, 4–5, 145n
border collie, 48, 96–97, 98
boredom, 216–17, 288–89
see also time, experience of
Bouvier des Flandres, 50
boxer, 51
brain
anatomy of olfaction and, 71–75
anatomy of vision and, 125, 126–32
big-brain hypothesis, 8–9, 44
breathing sounds, 103
breeds, dog, 47–55
American Kennel Club, 49, 50
ancient, 48–49
categories of, 49–52
characteristic traits of, 40, 42n, 47–52, 112, 114, 127–28,
287
closed gene pool of, 50–52
development of, 40, 48–49
differences in development, 47–48
"dog fancy" shows, 49n
genomes of, 50–52
modern, 49–50
mutts versus, 294
physical range, 43–44
purebred, 49–50, 51, 71
selective breeding, 40
threshold differences between, 52–55
briard, 50
bulldog, 114
bull terrier, 5
bumblebees, use of time, 215
bumps, 152
butyric acid, 20–21, 79
caching behavior, 210n, 226, 227,
227n
Cairn terrier, 50
camouflage, 123
cancer, detection of, 81–82
see also olfaction
Canidae family, 34, 38, 82–84
cats
attention in, 145
fetch and, 277
flehmen, 74
Cavalier King Charles spaniel, 114
character-reading, 168–70
chasing behaviors, 169–71
chatter, 103
chickadees, 185
see also imitation
chickens, spatial preferences of, 16–17
Chihuahua, 1–2, 124, 205–6
children
attention-getting in, 152, 153, 234
autism and, 140, 162, 192
developmental stages, 43, 141–44, 146, 162–63
epileptic seizure prediction in, 82n
fear of strange dogs and, 79–80
gaze bias of, 148n
guilt and, 230, 234
hearing abilities of, 93n
overimitation by, 188, 188n
rescue dogs and, 237–39
theory of mind, 192–93, 218, 234–35
see also infants, human
chimpanzees
animal cognition research on, 4–5, 102n, 145,
147, 148, 150–51, 157, 193
begging experiments with, 156
big-brain hypothesis and, 8–9
contagious yawns and, 280
human evolution and, 61–62, 62n
"mark" test of self-awareness, 218–19, 220
smile of, 16
chow chow, 3, 50
cilia, smell and, 71, 74
circadian rhythms, 88, 212–15, 227–28
see also time, experience of
cities and city design
grid-based, 86–87
human visual perception and, 136–37
life of urban dogs, 163, 283–85
in odor control, 86–87
paving, 86–87
personal space and, 17
sounds and, 251–52
street filth and, 56, 295
Clever Hans, 163–66
clicker training, 256n
see also training
cloned dogs, 52
clothing
anthropomorphism and, 17–19, 29, 56
dog height and, 244–45
odors on, 71, 75–76, 77, 78, 85, 244
raincoats for dogs, 17–19
coats for dogs, 17–19
cognition. see
animal cognition; physical cognition; social cognition
collie, 48, 96–97, 98
color vision, 128–29, 250, 252
see also vision
commands, 60, 96, 157–58, 171–72, 231,
231n
learning to obey, 182–83, 189–90, 204n,
285–86
communication, 89–119
asking dogs in, 26–28
barks as, 98, 105–8, 152–54, 171
of bats, 91, 241–43
body language in, 91, 108–14, 118
commands in. see commands
of dogs, 1–2, 5–6, 89–91, 108–18. see
also sounds made; urine
dog tells, 292–93
of elephants, 91
eye contact in, 45–47
of fireflies, 91–92
of frogs, 92
human vocalizations, 90–91
intentional, 115–18
keyboard use in, 145n
of monkeys, 91, 94
naming, 96–97, 98, 295–97
pitch of sounds made, 93–96, 98–108
pointing in, 49, 143, 150–52, 287
posture in, 80–81, 89–90, 108, 109–10, 111,
153 prosody in, 93–94, 95, 251
shaking in, 113–14
showing, 154
of socialized wolves, 63–64
urine as, 22, 82–84, 114–18
see also language
comparative psychology, 206–7
concealing behavior, 234–35
confirmation bias, 169n
conversational maxims, 97–98
coprophagia (feces eating), 73
cortisol, 172–73, 279–80 see also
hormones
countermarking, 117
counting ability, 163–66, 210
coursing dogs, 48
courting behavior, 83–84, 277–78
cows, slaughterhouse behavior of, 136, 251n
coyotes, 34
cries, 101
critical/sensitive period, 42–43, 222n, 262
cropping ears, 92
cross-species bond, 259–82
crouching, 110–11
cuteness, 264
dachshund, 40, 48, 53n
darting eyes, 148
Darwin, Charles, 29, 102n, 109
daydreaming, 88
deaf persons, dog companions for, 162, 240
death, 235–37
deception, 80, 169, 192–93, 201,
234–35
demonstration, 186–90
depression
in dogs, 14, 16, 26, 27, 112
in rats, 27–28
Derrida, Jacques, 281–82
dholes, 34
diabetes, detection of, 81
see also
olfaction
dingoes, 34
disease
bonding in prevention of, 279–80
olfaction in detection of, 81–82
DNA. see genes
Doberman, 51, 53n, 229–30
docked tails, 114
"dog fancy" shows, 49n
dog groomers, 85–86, 292
dog heroes. see emergencies
dog whistle, 93
dog years, 222n
dolphins
brain size of, 9n
self-awareness of, 219
smile of, 16
domestication, 33–65
archeological evidence of, 39–40
artificial selection in, 6, 35–37
definition of, 33–34
DNA evidence of, 39–40
dog-human bond and, 260
food preferences and, 33, 34
process of, 35–37, 38–47, 60–61, 64–65
of silver foxes, 35–37
theory of, 38–47
wolves and, 38–47, 55–61
dominance
body language showing, 110–11, 112,
114
in electric shock research, 19n
eye contact in, 147
hormones and, 172
licking and, 30–31
in marking behavior, 117
sounds made and, 102
in training dogs, 57–61
in wolves versus dogs, 18–19, 40, 147, 148
dopamine, 261
dreaming, 88, 124, 213, 213n
ducks
chasing behavior of, 141
greeting behavior of, 272
ears, 92–98, 109–10, 113
see also audition;
sounds made eating tone, 24
eavesdropping, with video camera, 291–92
echolocation, 91, 241–43
Egypt, dog breeds in ancient, 48–49
electric shock research, 19n
elephants
brain size of, 9n
communication of, 91
flehmen, 74
periscope sniff, 69
roses and, 22
self-awareness of, 219
Elizabethan collar, 86
emergencies
knowledge of, 240–41
rescue dogs, 79, 166, 237–39
stories of dog heroes, 237–39
test of behavior during, 239–41
encephalization quotient, 9n
endorphins, 279
English cocker spaniel, 50
epileptic seizure, prediction of, 82n
see also olfaction
exaggerated retreat, 203
eyebrows, raised as display, 16, 124
eye contact
aggression and, 147, 148–49
in attending, 143–44
avoiding, 46
dog tells, 292–93
dog vision and, 133–34
as foundation for social cognition,
45–47
gaze avoidance, 45–46, 110–11, 143, 144,
148–49, 229, 232, 292–93
gaze following, 149–52, 154
lack of, in autism, 140, 192
mutual gaze, 46–47, 139, 146–49, 161, 292–93
showing, 154
staring, 15, 45–46, 110, 292–93
use of gaze, 46
eyes. see also eye contact;
vision
darting, 148
of dogs versus wolves, 42
facial expressions, 110 eyebrows raised as
display, 16, 124
infant imitation of, 142–43, 173, 277
see also smile
facial recognition, 142–43
false belief task, 192–93, 201 Far Side (Larson), 95
fear
anal sacs and, 84–85
communication of, 99, 112
detection of, 79–81. see also
olfaction
in domestication of wolves, 37, 39
isolation barks and, 107
feces, 24, 73, 84–85, 295
feet
dog, 85
human, 71, 75–76, 78, 244
feral dogs, 59, 63, 83–84, 117–18
fetch, 277–78, 287
see also play, human-dog
Finnegan, 266–67, 301
fireflies, communication of, 91–92
flehmen, 74
flicker-fusion rate, 130–32
see also vision
flirting, timing in, 277–78
fluorescent lights, 131
foveae, 126–27
Fox, Michael, 269
foxes
in Canidae family, 34. see also Canidae family
domestication experiments, 35–37
vocalizations of, 90
free-ranging dogs, 59, 63, 83–84, 117–18
French bulldog, 114
friendly behavior, 171–72
frogs, communication of, 92
Fromm, Erich, 216
functional tone, 24–26
fur trade, domestication of silver foxes, 35–37
gaits, 290–91
galloping, 290
Gallup, Gordon, 217–20
gangs, dog-human, 57–61
gaze. see eye contact
generalizations, 8–9, 221–22
genes
divergence of dogs, 38–47
functioning of, 36–37
genetic mutations, 51
genome mapping, 36–37, 51, 52n
interaction with environment, 51–55
mitochondrial DNA, 39–40
of tamed foxes, 36–37
genitals, olfaction and, 75, 76, 87
German shepherd, 50, 53n, 104
golden retriever, 112
Goodall, Jane, 102n, 296
Grandin, Temple, 136, 251n
Great Dane, 50
greeting, 271–73
attachment behavior and, 43, 271–72 licking
in, 29, 30–31, 65, 247, 272–73
ritual as part of bond, 29, 30–31, 263–64,
271–73
sniff in, 75, 76, 84–85, 86, 87–88
tail in, 3, 31, 256–57
see also kisses, dog
greyhound, 48
Grice, Paul, 97–98
grin. see smile
growl, 62, 100, 102, 221–22. see also
sounds made
grunt, 101–2
guard dogs, 252
guilt, 228–35
associations and, 232–33
concealing behavior, 234–35
feeling of, 228–35
gaze avoidance and, 149, 229, 232
guilty look, 149, 228–30, 232, 233
lack of, 234
polygraph machines, 80, 169
test of, 230–33
habits, knowledge of, 166–72, 251–52
habituation, to odors, 70–71
hackles, 110, 113
half-bounding, 290–91
handedness/pawedness, 288
happiness, 26
licking and, 30–31
smiling and, 16, 145
wagging tail and, 112–13
Harlow, Harry, 268–69
harriers, 49
head bow, 201
head size, 8–9, 44, 124
hearing. see audition; sounds
made
heeling, 287–88
height, of dogs, 23, 40, 110n, 112, 169–71,
243–45
herding behavior, 49, 53, 54, 71, 262, 287
hierarchy
lack of, in domestic dogs, 57–61
of the senses, 133–34
in wolves, 40, 58–59
hippopotamus marking, 84
Homo sapiens
dog recognition of owner, 25–26, 29–31
in domestication process, 38–47
early settlements of, 38–39
evolution of, 40–41, 61–62
hormones
affected by petting dog, 261
contagion of in dogs, 80, 280
cortisol, 172–73, 279–80
detection by dogs, 80, 172–73, 280
detection by sharks, 77
endorphins, 279
oxytocin, 261, 279
pheromones and, 73–75, 79–81, 83
prolactin, 279
testosterone, 172
threshold levels of, 53
vasopressin, 261
horse
counting behavior of "Clever Hans,"163–66
pressure-release as reinforcement, 249n
hounds, 50
howl, 100, 101, 103
hunting
chasing behaviors and, 169–71
dogs bred for, 43, 49, 53–54
style of dogs, 43, 57, 57n, 59
style of wolves, 34, 38–40, 43, 57n, 58
in training dogs, 57–61
husky, 42n, 48, 50, 114, 135
hyenas, 34n
hypothalamus, 212–13
Ibizan Hound, 48–49n
imitation
in animal cognition research, 183–90
blue tit and chickadee behavior, 185
demonstration and, 186–90
"do as I do" task, 142–43, 186–90
of facial expressions, 142–43, 173, 277
learning through, 183–86
mirroring, 152–53
overimitation by children, 188, 188n
role in bonding process, 274–79
stimulus enhancement versus, 184–90
true imitation, 189–90
India, free-ranging dogs in, 83–84, 117–18
individuality
genetic differences and, 51–55
of humans versus dogs, 8–9
infants, human
attachment in, 43
bonding and, 267–69, 277
cuteness of, 264
developmental stages, 43, 141–44, 146
facial recognition by, 142–43
gaze following in, 150
imitation of smile, 143, 173, 277
motherese and, 95–96
object permanence and, 178–79
overimitation by, 188, 188n
visual perception in, 142
see also children
instincts, 236–37
intelligence, 175–207
brain size and, 8–9, 44
counting ability, 163–66, 210
dog intelligence tests, 175–81
learning from others, 176, 181–83
linguistic ability in, 94–95, 96–97
problem solving, 44–45, 175–81
tool use and, 44–45, 177
see also animal
cognition; physical cognition; social cognition
invisible displacement, test of, 177–78
in-your-face attention-getter, 152, 200, 203
irises, 125, 148
Irish setter, 50
Irish terrier, 50
isolation
eavesdropping, with video camera, 291–92
impact on bonding, 268–69
isolation barks, 107
lack of marking and, 116
noise making of dogs and, 118
jackals, 34
Jack Russell, 245
jaguars, 15
joint attention, 58
Kanzi (bonobo), 145n
keyboard use, 145n
Kipling, Rudyard, 119
kisses, dog, 29–31
see also licking
Koko (gorilla), 273 komondor, 113
Labrador retriever, 53, 127–28,
237–38
language
barks compared with, 105–6
commands. see commands
human use of, 26–27, 64, 94
linguistic ability of animals, 94–95, 96–97,
145n
role in memory, 224
vocabulary in, 96–97, 145n
Larson, Gary, 95
laughter, 103–4
learned helplessness, 19n
learned optimism/pessimism, 27–28
learning, 176, 181–83
associative, 10–11, 167–68, 182, 225, 232–33,
289
through demonstration, 186–90
through imitation, 183–86
to obey commands, 182–83, 189–90, 204n, 285–86
from observation, 26–28, 183–90
of umwelt of blind companion, 134–35, 162, 274
see also training
leg bands, 296n
licking
Elizabethan collar and, 86
functional use of, 29–31
as greeting, 29, 30–31, 65, 247, 272–73
muzzle licking, 30–31
in olfaction, 30–31, 73, 74
to prompt regurgitation by mother, 29–30, 272–73
sonorous mouth licking, 152
urine, 73, 74
of world, 246–47
lifetime, length of, 222n, 264
Linnaean classification, 38n
lizards, vomeronasal organ in, 73
looking. see vision
Lorenz, Konrad, 83, 116–17, 141n, 261–62,
272–73
marking
anal sacs in, 84–85, 112, 117
audience and, 117–18
"mark" test of self-awareness, 218–19, 220
scratching ground in, 85
urine in, 22, 76, 82–84, 114–18
markings, 296
marmosets, sniff in, 69
mastiff, 48–49, 50, 221
mating. see sexuality
mazes, 144
McClintock, Martha, 73n
memoirs, pet, 119, 225–27
memory, 222–28
associations and, 225
autobiographical memory, 119, 225–27
prodigious human memory, 224
willpower and, 226–27
mimicry, 94
mirror
"mark" test of self-awareness, 218–19, 220
recognition in, 217–20
mirroring, 152–53
mitochondrial DNA, 39–40
moan, 100, 101–2
mongoose
hyena relation to, 34n
marking by, 84
mongrels, 47, 52
monkeys
animal cognition research on, 157
anthropomorphism and, 263n
big-brain hypothesis and, 8–9
bonding and, 267–69
communication of, 91, 94
eyebrows raised in display, 16
eye contact in, 148
in zoos, 158–59
monosodium glutamate, 30
mothers
licking behavior and, 29–30, 272–73
motherese and, 95–96
touch between child and, 267–69
see also bonding
motion blindness, 132
motion sensitivity, 48, 54, 123–24, 129–32,
167, 244–45, 249, 250–51
movement, forms of, 290–91
mutts
adoption of, 47, 65, 262n, 266–67, 294, 301
muzzles of, 71
temperament of, 294
wolves versus, 44
mutual gaze, 46–47, 139, 146–49, 161,
292–93
muzzle biting, 18–19
muzzle licking, 30–31
Nagel, Thomas, 241–43, 258
naming behavior, 96–97, 98, 295–97
narcolepsy, 51
nasal septum, 74
natural selection, 34, 38–47
neophilia, 178n
neotony, 114, 264
neutering, 295
Newfoundland, 43–44, 48
night vision, 48, 125, 128–30
nonsporting hounds, 49
Norman (Labrador retriever), 237–38
Norwich terrier, 47–48
nose
anatomy of dog, 68–69, 71–75
beagle olfactory cells, 71, 72
of humans, 71
of sheepdog, 71
wet, in dogs, 69, 74–75, 77
see also odors;
olfaction; vomeronasal organ novelty, importance of, 284–85,
288–89
obedience, 182–83, 189–90, 204n, 285–86
see also right and wrong;
training
objectivity, in science, 3–5, 13–14,
27–28
object permanence, 176, 178–79
observation
in animal cognition research, 4–5, 13–14, 27–28, 102n, 145, 147,
148, 161–73
animal observation of humans, 183–90
anthropomorphism and, 14–19
learning from, 26–28, 183–90. see also imitation objectivity in scientific, 3–5,
13–14, 27–28 in training process, 60–61
of zoo animals, 4–5, 8, 14–15, 158–59,
216
odors
arousal and, 110
artificial, 25–26, 69, 71, 72, 86, 292
butyric acid and, 20–21, 79
city design and, 86–87
on clothing, 71, 75–76, 77, 78, 85, 244
of dog feet, 85
dog groomers and, 85–86, 292
in experience of time, 72, 77, 78–79, 227–28, 254–56
height of dogs and, 245
human adaptation to, 70–71
of human feet and shoes, 71, 75–76, 78, 135, 244
importance to dogs, 87–88
rump sniffing, 84–85, 86, 87–88
signature odor of humans, 25–26, 74–79, 87–88,
135 smell walks and, 284–85
traveling on air currents, 71, 76–77, 123,
245, 284–85
of urine, 22, 76, 82–84, 114–18
see also olfaction
olfaction, 67–88
bloodhound ability, 77–78
cells committed to, 69, 71, 72, 73–75
changes in, 169
detection of disease and disorder, 81–82
detection of fear, 79–81
detection of human odors, 20–21, 25–26, 76–77
in dogs, 30–31, 67–88, 116–17, 122
in experience of time, 72, 77, 78–79, 84, 227–28, 254–56
expression of anal sacs, 84–85
genitals and, 75, 76, 87
habituation in, 70–71
in humans, 21–22, 23, 67–68, 72–73, 122
importance to dogs, 87–88
licking in, 30–31, 73, 74
olfactory bulb, 72–73
pheromones and, 73–75, 79–81, 83
physiology of, 69, 71–75
prediction of seizure, 82n
self-awareness and, 219–20
in sharks, 77
signature scent and, 25–26, 74–79, 87–88, 135
smell walks and, 284–85
sniff in. see sniff
in ticks, 20–21
in tracking prey, 48, 57n, 76, 77–78
urine marking, 22, 76, 82–84, 114–18
vision connected with, 127–28
see also nose; odors; sniff;
vomeronasal organ
ontogenetic ritualization, 226n
open-mouth display, 108, 110, 201
operant conditioning, 10–11, 189–90
optic nerve, 135–36
optimism, learned, 27–28
orienting reflex, 141
owners
animal observation of, 161–73, 251–52
concept of, 11–12
dog recognition of, 25–26, 29–31
legal relationship with dogs, 11–12
theories about dogs, 3–4
training of dogs and, 10–11, 25–26
oxytocin, 261, 279 see
also hormones
pacing, 291
packs
analogy in training dogs, 57–61
use of term with dogs, 43, 57–61
in wolves, 38, 40, 41, 43, 57–61, 103 pair
bond, 260–61
panoramic vision, 124, 127–28
papillon, 43–44
paramecium, 236–37
Pavlov, Ivan, 289
pawedness/handedness, 288
pawing, 152
pecking, 185
pedomorphic dogs, 114
peek-a-boo, 142
Pekingese, 50
peripheral vision, 127–28
personality. see temperament
personal space of chickens, 16–17
of dogs, 246–47
of humans, 16–17
"territory marking" and, 83–84
of wolves, 38
pessimism, learned, 27–28
petting dogs, 266–71, 293
Pfungst, Oskar, 164
Pharaoh Hound, 48–49n
pheromones, 73–75, 79–81, 83
phonemes, 105–6
photoreceptors, 48, 126, 127–32, 168
physical cognition
attention in, 144
object displacement/permanence, 176, 177–79
rope task, 44–45
size-awareness in, 220–22
social cognition and, 176–81
tool use, 177
of wolves versus dogs, 44–45
Piaget, Jean, 162–63, 178
pigeons
"psychic powers" of, 165–66
use of time, 213, 215
pigs, slaughterhouse behavior of, 251n
pinna, 92
pit bull, 53n
pitch of sounds made, 93–96, 98–108
play, 196–205
aggression versus, 5, 61–65, 270
attention in, 152–53, 199–205
biting games, 42
cackling laugh in, 103–4
commands during, 171–72
communication in, 1–2, 5–6, 200–205
description of, 1–2, 5–6, 205–6, 277–78
dog love of, 289
between dogs of mismatched size, 1–2, 124, 205–6, 221
of domesticated silver foxes, 37
fetch and, 277–78, 287
function of, 197–98
human-dog, 277–78, 287
objects in, 24–26, 247–50
ritualization of, 198
role in bonding process, 275–79
rolling behavior in, 24, 55, 85, 286
rough-and-tumble, 1–2, 24, 62, 196–98, 200n, 202, 205–6,
221
study of, 196–205
theory of mind and, 196–205
visual streak and ball dogs, 127–28, 287
see also attention-getter; play
signals
play barks, 98–99, 108
play bow, 201, 203, 205
play signals, 199–207, 226
head bow, 201
importance of, 200–201, 289
open-mouth display, 108, 110, 201
play bow, 201, 203, 205
play slap, 103, 201
play slap, 103, 201
Pliny the Elder, 64–65
pointing
as communication, 49, 143, 150–52, 287
gaze following as, 150–52, 154
temporal patterns in, 277–78
polygraph machines, 80, 169
poodle, 42n, 240
posture
antithesis and, 110, 112
as communication, 80–81, 89–90, 108, 109–10,
111, 153 tail and, 111–14
prairie voles, 261
prey, chasing behaviors and, 169–71
primates
animal cognition research on, 4–5, 102n
big-brain hypothesis and, 8–9
problem-solving skills
imitation and, 183–90
intelligence and, 44–45, 175–81
of wolves versus dogs, 180
progressive mutilation, 229
prolactin, 279
prosody, 93–94, 95, 251
protodogs, 42
protodomesticators, 40
"psychic powers,"163–72
animal knowledge of habits versus, 166–72, 251–52
confirmation bias, 169n
in counting behavior of horse, 163–66
of pigeons, 165–66
in spelling behavior of dogs, 167
see also attention
pug, 40, 50, 112, 127, 221
Pumpernickel (Pump)
adoption of, 65
anecdotes, 4–6, 13, 29, 31, 33, 34, 52, 55,
67, 71, 75, 85, 87, 89, 92, 98, 100–101,
104, 108, 111–12, 113, 114–15, 118–19, 121,
123–24, 132, 135, 139, 166–67,
169–71, 183, 184, 190–91, 196–97, 199, 209,
211, 215, 222–23, 225, 228,
229, 235, 241, 247, 256–57, 259, 271–72, 274,
276, 283–85, 291, 294
described, 3, 47, 299–301
punishment
historical types of, 228–29
scolding in, 149, 232–33
in training process, 60–61, 286
pupil size, 124–25
pups
cuteness of, 264
lack of urine marking, 117
licking behavior of mother, 29–30,
272–73
sounds made by, 101, 102
in wolf packs, 58–59
purebred dogs, 49–50, 51, 71
raincoats, dog, 17–19
raised-leg display, 83, 116, 117–18
see also urine
raisin toxicity, 33, 34, 34n
rats
big-brain hypothesis and, 8–9
fear contagion in, 80
learned optimism and pessimism, 27–28
sounds made by, 93
use of time, 213, 215
redirected appeasement ceremony (Lorenz), 272–73
reflexes, 141
REM sleep, 213n
reptiles, vomeronasal organ in, 73
rescue dogs, 79, 166, 237–39
retina, 125, 126–32, 135–36, 249
retrieving behavior, 49, 53, 54, 112, 127–28, 154, 287
rewards, in training process, 60–61
rhinoceros, 4–5, 15n
flehmen, 74
urine marking, 84
Rico (border collie), 96–97, 98, 211n
right and wrong, knowledge of, 228–35
rolling behavior, 24, 55, 85, 86, 286
rolling tones, 24
rooting reflex, 141, 269
rope task, 44–45
roses, 22, 72
Rottweiler, 53n
rough-and-tumble play, 1–2, 24, 62, 196–98,
200n, 202, 205–6, 221 rump sniffing, 84–85, 86, 87–88
saccades, 136
Sacks, Oliver, 162
salty taste, 30
San Diego Zoo, 4–5
scavengers, 39, 40, 43, 63
schizophrenia, smell of, 81
sclera, 125
SCN (suprachiasmatic nucleus), 212–13
scolding, 149, 232–33
scream, 101
scrub jay, caching behavior of, 226
Seeing Eye dogs, 43, 134–35, 152, 162, 274
seizure prediction, 82n
selection
artificial, 6, 35–37, 41. see also domestication natural, 34, 38–47
selective breeding, 40
self-awareness
experience of, 217–22
knowledge of right and wrong, 228–35
"mark" test of, 218–19, 220
olfaction and, 219–20
size and, 220–22
of working dogs, 221–22
self-takedown, 221
sensitive/critical period, 42–43, 222n,
262
separation anxiety, 43, 268n, 271–72, 280
sexuality
courting behavior, 83–84, 277–78
and domestication of silver foxes, 36
flehmen and, 74
flirting and, 277–78
genetic mutations and, 51
genitals and, 75, 76, 87
hormones and, 172
neutering dogs, 295
raised-leg display in, 83, 116, 117–18
of wolves, 38, 41, 58
shake, body, 113–14
shaping behavior, 10–11
shark, olfaction of, 77
shar-pei, 50
sheepdog
early exposure to sheep, 221, 262
"eye" behavior, 287
innate behavioral tendencies, 54
nose of, 71
shepherding behavior. see herding
behavior
shepherds, 49, 50, 53, 53n, 104
shoes
for dogs, 56
odor of, 71, 75–76, 78, 135, 244
showing, 154
shriek, 103
Siberian husky, 42n, 48, 50, 114, 135
sighing, 27, 153
sight. see vision
signature odor, 25–26, 74–79, 87–88, 135
sitting tone, 24
size
big-dog sounds, 102
breed differences, 43–44
height in, 23, 40, 110n, 112, 169–71, 243–45
of objects in relation to dogs, 247–50
recognition of own, 220–22
in rough-and-tumble play, 1–2, 124, 205–6, 221
toy dogs, 49, 290–91
slaughterhouses, 136, 251n
sledding, 169–71
sleep
circadian rhythms and, 88, 212–15
dreaming and, 88, 124, 213, 213n
smelling. see nose; odors;
olfaction
smile
of chimpanzees, 16
of dogs, 16, 110, 256–58, 273
of dolphins, 16
happiness and, 16, 145 infant imitation of,
143, 173, 277
snarl, 102
sneeze, 68
sniff, 68–71
characterized by ethologists, 69–70, 83–84
description of, 67, 68, 69–70
in dogs, 67, 68–71, 83–88, 116–17
in elephants, 69
in greeting, 75, 76, 84–85, 86, 87–88
in humans, 70–71
in marmosets, 69
rump sniffing, 84–88
in tortoises, 69
types of, 67, 68
wet nose good for, 69, 74–75, 77
see also nose; odors;
olfaction
Snoopy, 226
snuffling, 103
social cognition
animal behavior in zoos, 4–5, 8, 14–15, 15n, 158–59, 216
animal knowledge of humans, 161–73
animal preferences and, 16–17
attention in, 144–45
in detection of fear, 79–81
in domestication of dogs, 38–47
eye contact and, 45–47
linguistic ability of animals, 94–95
packs and, 38, 40, 41, 43, 57–61, 103
physical cognition and, 176–81
play and, 1–2, 5–6, 206–7. see also
play
"psychic powers" of animals, 163–72
sensitive/critical period of, 43
see also attention;
communication
social panting, 103–4
socks, 244
Sofia (dog), 145n
Sony, 276
sounds made, 98–108 bark. see barks
breathing, 103
cackling laugh, 103–4
chatter, 103
context and, 99–100
cry, 101
duration of, 100
growl, 62, 100, 102, 221–22
grunt, 101–2
high-frequency, 93, 101
howl, 100, 101, 103
method of production, 100
moan, 100, 101–2
noisy breathing, 103
pitch of, 93–96, 98–108
play slap, 103, 201
range of hearing in dogs, 92, 93–94
range of hearing in humans, 21–22, 23–24, 92–94
scream, 101
shriek, 103
sigh, 27, 153
snarl, 102
sneeze, 68
snuffling, 103
social panting, 103–4
sonorous mouth licking, 152
squeal, 101
ultrasonic, 91, 93, 241–43
whimper, 101
whine, 100, 101, 153
yawn, 110, 280–81
yelp, 98, 100–101
sour taste, 30
spaniel, 3, 49, 50, 114
species, nature of, 38n
spelling, 167
sporting hounds, 49
springer spaniel, 3
squeal, 101
standing over, 18–19
Stanley (Jack Russell), 245
staring, 15, 45–46, 110, 292–93
Stein, Gertrude, 161
Sterbak, Jana, 245
stimulus enhancement, 184–90
strange dogs, 79n
stranger barks, 107
stray dogs, 63
stress response
cortisol in, 172–73, 279–80
eye contact and, 45–46
in fear, 79–81
genes and, 36–37
sounds in, 104
tails in, 112
yawn in, 110
stroboscopic vision, 131 submission
body language showing, 110–11, 112,
114
in electric shock research, 19n
eye contact in, 147
licking and, 30–31
in training dogs, 57–61
in wolves versus dogs, 18–19
suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), 212–13
surrogate mothers, 268–69
sweat, 20–21, 75, 76, 80
sweet taste, 30
synchrony
of assistance dogs, 43, 134–35, 162, 274 in
regulation of time, 274–79
tail
docked, 114
in greetings, 3, 31, 256–57
meaning of wags, 3, 31, 112–13 use of,
111–14
tapetum lucidum,
125
taste
importance of, 122
reception, 30
types of flavors, 30
teeth
showing, 110
tooth-snapping, 100
television-watching, 157
flicker-fusion rate and, 131–32
in observing animals, 14
see also vision
tells, dog, 292–93
temperament, 47–48, 55
termites, sounds made by, 93
terriers, 5, 47–48, 49, 50, 53n, 56
territory marking, 83–84
testosterone, 172
see also hormones
theory of mind, 190–207
children and, 192–93, 218, 234–35
comparative psychology and, 206–7
concept of, 190–92
dog mind and, 193–206
false belief task, 192–93, 201
nonverbal test of, 192–93
play and, 196–205
rudimentary theory of mind, 204–5
self-awareness and, 217–22
thermotactile sensory probe (Fox), 269
threshold differences, 52–55
ticks, deer, 20–22
time, 211–17, 251–56
boredom and, 216–17, 288–89
bumblebee use of, 215
circadian rhythms, 88, 212–15, 227–28
dog "pacemaker" and, 212–15
dog years and, 222n
memories and, 119, 222–28
odors and, 72, 77, 78–79, 84, 227–28, 254–56
regulation of, 274–79
temporal patterns in bonding, 274–79
time-telling by dogs, 227–28
Tinbergen, Niko, 147n
tool use, 44–45, 177
tooth-snapping, 100
tortoises, sniff in, 69
touch
as element in bonding, 266–71, 293
between mother and child, 267–69
petting, 266–67, 293
sense of, 122, 269–71 see also
licking
toy dogs, 49, 290–91
toys
ball retrieval, 126–32, 154
color vision and, 128–29
dog beds and, 25
dog naming of, 96–97, 98
object displacement and, 177–79
size of objects, 247–50
tracking, 48, 57n
olfaction in, 76, 77–78
style of dogs, 48, 57n
style of wolves, 57n
vision in, 123–24, 143
training
associative learning in, 10–11, 167–68, 182
clickers in, 256n
dogs' reading of trainers in, 166–72
domestication of dogs and, 33–34
for emergencies, 240
guidelines for, 285–86
learning from others, 176, 181–83
to obey commands, 182–83, 189–90, 204n,
285–86
by owners, 10–11, 25–26 pack analogy and,
57–61
"psychic powers" of animals, 163–72,
251–52
punishment versus reward in, 60–61, 286
for scent recognition, 76, 77–78, 81–82
successes in, 11
wolf versus dog behavior and, 55–61
see also learning
trotting, 290
tuberculosis, detection of, 81
typhoid fever, detection of, 81
ultrasonic sounds, 91, 93, 241–43
umami taste, 30
umwelt
"acting into,"23#8211;24
anthropomorphism versus, 14–17, 23, 31–32, 263n, 294–96
of blind companion, 134–35, 162, 274
de-animalizing of dogs, 295
defined, 20
of dogs, 13–19, 22, 23, 24–26, 31–32, 132–37, 241–58,
287–88
functional tones and, 24–26
of humans, 21–22, 24–26, 93n
impact of dogs on human, 283–84
of ticks, 20–22
unfriendly behavior, 171–72
uniformity of species, 8–9
urine
as communication, 22, 82–84, 114–18
in detection of cancer, 81–82
ground scratching associated with, 85
hippopotamus marking, 84
licking, 73, 74
marking behavior, 22, 76, 82–84, 114–18
mongoose marking, 84
odor of, 22, 76, 82–84, 114–18
pheromones in, 73, 74, 83
punishment associated with, 60–61
raised-leg display, 83, 116, 117–18 rhinoceros marking, 84
vasopressin, 261 see
also hormones
veterinarian, smell of fear and,
84–85
video cameras, 5–6, 113, 198–99, 231–32, 245,
291–92
vision, 121–37
anatomy of dog eye, 124–32
animal behavior affected by, 15n, 126–32
animal observation of humans, 161–73
area centralis, 127
balls and, 126–32, 154
color vision, 128–29, 250, 252
dog companions for blind persons, 43, 134–35, 152, 162,
274
dog visual field, 135–37
flicker-fusion rate, 130–32
foveae in, 126–27
human visual perception, 21–22, 67–68, 72–73,
135–37, 141–44 lateral versus panoramic, 124, 127–28
motion sensitivity and, 48, 54, 123–24,
129–32, 167, 244–45, 249, 250–51
night vision, 48, 125, 128–30
photoreceptors, 48, 126, 127–32, 168
purpose of, 122–24
retina in, 125, 126–32, 135–36, 249
sclera, 125
tapetum lucidum, 125
television-watching, 14, 131–32, 157
in tracking prey, 123–24, 143
visual attention, 139
visual streak, 127–28, 287
of wolves, 123, 125
see also eye contact; eyes
vocabulary
of birds, 145n
of dogs, 96–97
voles, 261 vomeronasal organ, 122–23
in animals, 73–75, 79
as explanation for wet nose, 69, 74–75,
77
in humans, 74
pheromone detection in, 73–75
von Uexküll, Jakob, 20–22, 24
wagging tails, 3, 31, 112–13
walking, 169–71
allelometric behavior, 275
described, 290
smell walks, 284–85
weaning, 262n
whales
brain size of, 9n
communication of, 91
whimper, 101
whine, 100, 101, 153
whiskers, 67
whistle, dog, 93
Whiten, Andrew, 188n
Wild Animal Park (Escondido), 4–5, 15n
wild side of dogs, 61–64
willpower, 226–27
Wilson, Edward O., 265n
Wittgenstein, Ludwig, 253
wolfhound, 1–2, 124, 205–6
wolves
attachment in, 63–64
behavior of, 29–30, 45, 89–90
Canidae family and, 34, 38
developmental differences between dogs and, 10, 42–45, 50–52,
55–64
divergence of, 38–47
DNA differences between dogs and, 39–40, 62
domestication of, 38–47, 55–61
dominance and submission in, 18–19, 40, 147, 148
hunting behavior of, 34
hunting by, 38–40, 43, 57n, 58
packs in, 38, 40, 41, 43, 57–61, 103
physical cognition tasks and, 44–45
physical differences between dogs and, 43–44
problem-solving by, 180
as scavengers, 39, 40, 43
socialization of, 38–47, 63–64
social organization of, 38, 40, 41
tails of, 112
touch and, 269
vision of, 123, 125
Woolf, Virginia, 119 working dogs
assistance dogs, 43, 134–35, 152, 162, 240,
274
companions for blind, 43, 134–35, 152, 162, 274
guard dogs, 252
herders, 49, 53, 54, 71, 262, 287
hunters, 43, 49, 53–54
linguistic ability of, 95
rescue dogs, 79, 166, 237–39
self-awareness of, 221–22
trackers, 76, 77–78, 79
yard dogs, 180 yawn
contagious, 280–81
as sign of stress, 110
yelp, 98, 100–101
zebra finches, leg band preference in, 296n
zoos
animal behavior in, 4–5, 8, 14–15, 158–59, 216
petting, 267
About the Author
Alexandra Horowitz
earned her B.A. in philosophy from the University of Pennsylvania
and a Ph.D. in cognitive science from the University of California
at San Diego, studying dog cognition. She is currently a term
assistant professor of psychology at Barnard College and continues
to research dog behavior. In addition to her work with dogs, she
has also studied cognition in humans, rhinoceroses, and bonobos.
She previously worked as a lexicographer at Merriam-Webster and a
fact-checker for The New Yorker. She
lives in New York City with her husband and Finnegan, a dog of
indeterminate parentage and determinate character, and the fond
memories of dogs past.
She also likes to sketch her dogs.
Of course, researchers soon found brains
bigger than ours: the dolphin's brain is larger, as are the brains
of physically larger creatures such as whales and elephants. The
"big brain" myth has long been overturned. Those who are still
interested in mapping brain to smarts now look at other, more
sophisticated measures: the amount of convolution of the brain; the
encephalization quotient, a ratio that includes both brain and body
size in the calculation; the quantity of neocortex; or the gross
number of neurons and synapses between neurons.
This was made most evident for me one day
collecting data of the behavior of the white rhinoceros. At the
Wild Animal Park it is the animals who roam (relatively) freely,
and the visitors are restricted to trains that travel around the
large enclosures. I was situated in the narrow patch of grass
between the track and the fence, watching a typical day of rhino
socializing. As the trains approached, the rhinos stopped what they
were doing and moved quickly into a defensive huddle: standing with
rumps together, heads radiating out in a rough sunburst. The
animals are peaceful, but with poor vision they can be easily
startled if they do not smell someone approaching, and they count
on each other as lookouts. The train stopped, and everyone gaped at
the rhinos who, it was announced by the guide, were "doing
nothing." Eventually the driver moved on, and the rhinos resumed
their ordinary behavior.
This is similar to what was discovered by
midcentury behaviorist researchers who exposed laboratory dogs to
an electric shock from which they couldn't escape. Later, put in a
chamber from which there was a visible escape route, and shocked
again, these dogs showed learned
helplessness: they did not try to avoid
the shock by escaping. Instead, they froze in place, seemingly
resigned to their fate. The researchers had essentially trained the
dogs to be submissive and accept their lack of control of the
situation. (They later forced the dogs to unlearn the response and
end the shock.) Happily, the days of experiments wherein we shock
dogs to learn about their responses are over.
Not on this list are hyenas. Dog-sized and -shaped, with erect German
shepherd–like ears, and prone to howl and vocalize like many
garrulous canids, hyenas are in some ways doglike, but are not in
fact canids. They are carnivores more closely related to mongooses
and cats than to dogs.
†
Raisins are now suspected of being toxic to
some dogs, even in small amounts (though the mechanism of toxicity
is unknown)—leading me to wonder whether Pump was instinctively
averse to raisins.
What (some) genes do is regulate the formation
of proteins that assign cells their roles. When, where, and in what
environment a cell develops all contribute to the result. Thus the
path from a gene to the emergence of a physical trait or a behavior
is more circuitous than one might initially think, with room for
modifications along the way.
There is some debate over whether dogs should
be considered a separate species from, or a subspecies of wolves.
There is even debate over whether the original Linnaean
classification scheme that demarcates species as a fundamental unit is still helpful or
valid. Most researchers agree that describing wolves and dogs as
separate species is the best current description. Although the two
animals can inter-breed, their typical mating habits, their social
ecology, and the environments they live in are very
different.
Mitochondrial DNA are chains of DNA within the
energy-producing mitochondria of cells, but outside the cell
nucleus. They are inherited, without any change, from the mother by
her offspring. The mtDNA of individuals has been used to trace
human ancestry, and to estimate the evolutionary relationships
among animal species.
There is also a large breed difference. For
instance, poodles don't show avoidance behaviors or begin to
play-fight until weeks after huskies do—when weeks represent a
goodly chunk of the puppy's life. In fact, huskies develop more
quickly than wolves in some ways. No one has studied how this
affects their rapport with humans.
As the domestication process probably began
with early canids scavenging around human groups—eating our table
scraps—it is a particularly silly stance to feed dogs only raw
meat, on the theory that they are wolves at heart. Dogs are
omnivores who for millennia have eaten what we eat. With very few
exceptions, what is good on my plate is good for my dog's
bowl.
Temperament is
used to mean roughly the same thing as personality, without the overtone of
anthropomorphism. It is perfectly acceptable to talk about a dog's
personality, if we mean the dog's "usual pattern of behavior and
individual traits": behavior and traits are not exclusive to
humans. Some researchers use temperament to refer to the traits as they appear
in a young animal—the genetic tendency of the dog; while reserving
personality to refer to adult traits
and behaviors, the result of that particular temperament combined
with whatever they confronted in their environment.
There is no evidence, however, that any
currently existing breed can lay claim to being the descendents of
the original breeds. Descriptions of both the Pharaoh and Ibizan
hounds cite them as the "oldest" dog breeds, which claims seemed
supported by their physical resemblance to the dogs of Egyptian
paintings. However, their genomes reveal them to have emerged much
more recently.
The named occupation is mostly theoretical
because a minority of dogs bred for work actually do the work of
their breed (predominantly hunting or herding). The rest wind up
either as companions sitting on our laps, or trained, trimmed, and
blown-dry to be shown at "dog fancy" shows—odd, as nibbling the
crusts of our sandwiches after a nice shampoo is about as far as
one can get from dredging fallen waterfowl from a swamp.
Genetic analysis tests have become available
since the mapping of the genome: for a fee, companies will
allegedly resolve your dog's genetic code, determined from a blood
sample or swab of cheek cells, into its contributing breeds. At
present the accuracy of the tests is indeterminate.
What is considered aggressive is culturally and generationally
relative. German shepherds were on the top of the list after World
War II; in the 1990s Rottweilers and Dobermans were scorned; the
American Staffordshire terrier (also known as the pit bull) is the
current bête noire. Their classification has more to do with recent
events and public perception than with their intrinsic nature.
Recent research found that of all breeds, dachshunds were the most aggressive to both their
own owners and to strangers. Perhaps this is underreported because
a snarling dachshund can be picked up and stashed away in a tote
bag.
Not only do dogs not typically hunt to feed
themselves—whether encouraged to or not—but what hunting technique
they have is, it has been noted, "sloppy." A wolf makes a calm,
steady track toward his prey, without any frivolous moves;
untrained dogs' hunting walks are herky-jerky, meandering back and
forth, speeding and slowing. Worse, they may get waylaid by
distracting sounds or a sudden urge to playfully pursue a falling
leaf. Wolves' tracks reveal their intent. Dogs have lost this
intent; we have replaced it with ourselves.
Notably, the number of behavioral similarities
between chimpanzees and humans (culture and language aside for the
moment) increases steadily as the number of scientific studies of
the chimps also steadily increases.
… theoretically: no swimming pools have been
used in such a test. Instead, experimenters use extremely small
samples of an odorless medium, and then add an even more extremely
small sample of sugar to one of them.
The psychologist Martha McClintock was the
first to seriously study pheromone detection in humans; she and
others have done savvy, fascinating studies of how our behavior and
hormonal rates may be affected by pheromones or pheromone-like
hormones. But the jury is still out—and loudly arguing—on these
claims.
This construction—strange
dog—itself seems geared to inspire fear. Its use is also
based on a flawed premise: that familiar dogs will behave
predictably and reliably, and unfamiliar ones will not. As we've
seen, as much as we may want dogs to behave in lockstep with our
desires, their simply being their own animals ensures that they
will not always do so.
Research on other diseases is proceeding
apace. Provocatively, dogs who live in homes with epileptics seem
to be moderately good predictors of seizures. Two studies report
that dogs licked the person's face or hands, whimpered, stood
nearby, or moved protectively—in one case sitting on a child, and in another blocking a child's
access to stairs—before seizures. If this is true, there may be
olfactory, visual, or some other invisible (to us) cues that the
dogs used. But as the data come from "self-report"—family
questionnaires rather than data gathered more objectively—more
evidence is needed. We can however, pause in admiration of the
possibility of such a skill.
In reality, few people hear equally well
across this spectrum. With age, the higher-frequency sounds, above
11–14 kilohertz, go undetected by the human ear. This knowledge
prompted the inspired design of a product with the teenager's
umwelt in mind. The device emits a 17 kilohertz tone—out of the
range of most adults' hearing, but unpleasantly audible to
youngsters. Shop owners have used it as a teenager repellent, to
discourage loitering around their businesses.
Since the publication of Rico's successes, in
2004, other dogs (also border collies, for the most part) have been
reported with vocabularies from eighty to over three hundred words:
all names for various toys. You might have one of these prodigious
vocabularians in your house.
Except when it is animated: a discarded
plastic bag being tumbled down a city sidewalk by a breeze can
provoke growls, caution, and occasional attacks by alarmed dogs.
Dogs can be animistic, just as humans are in infancy: trying to
make sense of the world by attributing a familiar quality (of life)
to unknown objects. My plastic-bag-growling dog is in good company:
Darwin described his own dog treating an open parasol moving in the
breeze as a living thing, barking at and stalking it. And Jane
Goodall has observed chimpanzees making threatening gestures toward
thunderclouds. I've been known to fulminate thundercloud-ward
myself.
Surprisingly, dogs mind each other's posture
more than their height: dogs do not read simply being taller as
being dominant or confident. As we'll see later, it's not quite
right to say, as is often said of a bravely forward small dog,
he thinks he's a big dog: Actually, he
does not—he knows it is posture that matters.
The conversion to entirely digital television
broadcasts will eliminate the flicker-fusion problem, making
TV-viewing more viable (but no more olfactorily interesting) for
dogs—who are no doubt ambivalent.
Ethologist Konrad Lorenz beautifully
demonstrated this tendency of young waterfowl in the 1930s by
positioning himself as that first adult creature seen by a gaggle
of greylag goslings. They followed him readily, and Lorenz wound up
raising the brood as his own.
Developmental psychologists rely on the fact
that though infants cannot report what they are thinking, they
reliably look longer at things that interest them. By using this
one feature of infant behavior, psychologists gather data about
what the infant can see, distinguish, and understand and what he
prefers.
Well, for the most part: Kanzi the bonobo and
Alex the African gray parrot are among those who have been asked
and have answered: Alex was able to create and utter novel,
coherent, three-word sentences based on a vocabulary built from
eavesdropping on researchers; Kanzi has a multihundred-word
vocabulary of lexigrams (symbolic pictures) that he can point at to
communicate. And a single dog, Sofia, has been trained to use a
simple eight-key keyboard concurrent with events she had already
learned, like going for a walk, going into a crate, and getting
food or a toy. She learned to press the appropriate key to make a
request. As a communication, this behavior is closer to asking for
dinner by bringing an owner an empty food bowl than it is to a
full-fledged language. More abstract utterances have not been
reported (nor abstract keyboards designed).
One could make an argument that this behavior
was reinforced because of the survival value of looking at humans.
As with infants, an adult face will hold much information, not the
least of which could be where the next meal is coming from. The
early-twentieth-century ethologist Niko Tinbergen similarly found
that baby gulls have a strong attraction to the red-dotted beaks of
adult gulls (and to any stick with a red dot placed on it by an
ethologist, too).
Dogs show an additional tendency, one that
people do, too, when looking at faces: to look leftward first (that
is, to the right side of the face). Even young children show this
"gaze bias": looking first, and longer, to the right side of an
examined face. By closely observing dogs observing faces,
researchers have found that dogs share this bias—when looking at
human faces. When looking at other dogs, they show no gaze bias at
all. Why this might be is still a matter of conjecture: perhaps we
express emotions differently on each hemi-face; and perhaps dogs
emote more symmetrically (lopsided ears aside). Dogs have learned
to look at humans the way humans look at humans.
It should be noted that this skill is affirmed
by dogs' following a pointing hand to one of two baited buckets at
rates "significantly above chance." What this means is that they
don't choose which bucket to search under first randomly. Instead
they choose the bucket pointed at from 70 to 85 percent of the
time. That's good, but they are still making the wrong judgment 15
to 30 percent of the time! Three-year-old children get the right
bucket every single time. What this indicates is that the dogs'
success is probably the result of a mode of understanding that is
not identical to ours.
Dogs who often wind up being called
people dogs for their keen interest in
owners over dogs.
Spelling the word instead of saying it is, of
course, usually futile. Dogs can also learn the connection between
the cadence of a spelled word and a subsequent walk, even if the
latter does not immediately follow the former. On the other hand,
used in an unlikely context—say, sitting in the bath—the spelled
word will not evoke much interest. Chances are slim you're about to
up and take a walk when naked and sudsy.
While dogs may in fact distinguish between
people behaving subtly differently, one suspects that anyone using
their dogs this way might be susceptible to what psychologists call
confirmation bias: noticing just that
part of their dog's response that supports their own theories about
the person. Does the gentleman seem a bit untrustworthy to you? And
yes, look how your dog growled at him once: that settles it. Dogs
become amplifiers of our own beliefs; we can attribute to them that
which we think ourselves.
Dogs have a preference for novel
objects—neophilia. One study found that
when asked to retrieve an unspecified toy from a pile of familiar
and new toys, dogs spontaneously chose the new toys over
three-quarters of the time. This penchant for the new might explain
why when two dogs carrying sticks meet in a park, they often
simultaneously drop the stick they've been proudly toting around in
order to try to grab the pride of the approaching dog.
The dogs' ambivalence about the scent trail
may at first seem surprising, given all our talk about their
olfactory skills. But simply being able to smell a trail doesn't
mean that they use this ability all the
time. Often dogs need to be trained to be attentive to particular
scents.
My favorite example of the child's
overimitation comes from an experiment that psychologist Andrew
Whiten and his colleagues ran using a locked box with a tempting
piece of candy inside. They were curious if three-to five-year-old
children could imitate the particular means experimenters
demonstrated to unlock the box (involving twisting out rods fit
through barrel openings). The children watched, captivated, and
were then handed the re-locked box. Whiten found that the children
almost all imitated—and the youngest children over imitated—twisting the rod not two or three but
sometimes hundreds of times before pulling it out. What they did
not yet understand was exactly what part of the (twisting) means
was necessary to get the (candy-yielding) end.
Given the importance of regular visual
assurances that the game is still a game, it is perhaps not
surprising that successful three-way rough-and-tumble play is much
rarer than play between two dogs. As with conversation, something
is missed—a play signal here, an attention-getter there—when
everyone is speaking at once. Typically, only dogs familiar with
each other pull off threesomes.
Another indication of the dog's perception of
fairness comes from a new experiment demonstrating that dogs who
see another dog getting a reward for doing an act—shaking a paw on
command—but who do not themselves get rewarded for the same act
eventually refuse to shake anymore. (No rewarded dog was moved by
the clear injustice of the situation to share his earned bounty
with his unlucky partner, though …)
As when she spent a quarter hour digging a
hole in which to drop a treasured rawhide chew, but in digging
actually created more of a pile than a hole: the result being that
the rawhide was actually not stashed secretly away, but proudly and
conspicuously displayed (itself probably the result of an
imperfectly-honed caching instinct). In like manner, one might
wonder if she experiences it as ironic (or as magic) when I make a
show of unfurling my fingers in front of her and the treat I had in
my palm is missing.
This could be another way of accounting for
Rico's ability to pick the toy with the unfamiliar name out of a
pile of toys: he selected the toy that he did not
recognize.
With age, dogs sleep more but enter
paradoxical—REM—sleep less than in youth. Scientists have theories
but no final explanation for why dogs dream—and they dream vividly,
if their eye fluttering, claw curling, tail twitching, and yelping
in sleep is any indication. As in humans, one theory names dreams
the accidental result of paradoxical sleep, which itself is a time
of bodily restoration; alternatively, dreams might function as a
time to practice, in the safety of one's imagination, future social
interactions and physical feats or to review interactions and feats
past.
When animals pass the test, skeptics highlight
the logical fallacy of the conclusion: that self-aware humans use a
mirror to examine ourselves does not imply that using a mirror
requires self-awareness. When animals fail the test, the debate
goes the other way: there is no good evolutionary reason why
animals should examine something
non-irritating on their heads, even if they recognized themselves.
In either event, the mirror test continues to be the best test thus
far developed for self-awareness, and one that uses simple
equipment to boot.
I do not know if the origin of the myth of
dog years—that dogs live the equivalent
of seven years for every one of our years—has ever been cracked.
I'd guess that it is a backward extrapolation from the length of
the expected lifetime of humans (seventy+ years) to the expected
lifetime of dogs (ten to fifteen). The analogy is more convenient
than it is true. There is no real life-length equivalence except
that we both are born and die. Dogs develop at lightning speed,
walking and eating on their own in their first two months; human
infants take over a year. By a year, most dogs are accomplished
social actors, able to navigate dog and human worlds easily. The
average child might be there by four or five. Then dog development
slows, while human development skyrockets. If committed to the
comparison, one could make a case for a sliding scale ratio: around
10 to 1 in their first two years, then diminishing to more like 2
to 1 in their last years. But the truly committed should consider
the critical-period windows, the performance on cognitive tests,
the diminishment of sensory capacities with age, and the lifespans
of different breeds in their calculus.
This is similar to what has been called
ontogenetic ritualization: the
co-shaping by individuals of a behavior over time, until even the
very initial part of the behavior carries meaning for them. In
humans, an eyebrow-raise from one friend to another can take the
place of a spoken commentary; as we've seen, among dogs a quick
head-raise might replace an entire play bow.
Which some wolves instinctively do: even as
young cubs they burrow their noses into a patch of land, drop a
bone, nose-burrow some more, then proudly leave their poor excuse
of a hole with a bone obviously visible. As adults they refine the
behavior and do retrieve cached food—although there is no data
about whether the retrieval is time-sensitive.
The medieval policy seems ridiculous to
presume that dogs merit lawful consideration. It may seem equally
ridiculous that our modern policy presumes that dogs do not: we
still kill dogs who mortally wound a human—but now we call the dogs
"dangerous" and do not bother to put them on trial (though their
owners might be tried).
The command varies from owner to owner—from
no! to the recently popular
leave it! Each is fundamentally a
negation: a sharp-sounding grammatical flourish that can be applied
concurrently to any behavior to make it off-limits.
For a horse, releasing pressure on the body is
sufficiently pleasurable as to be able to be used as a
reinforcement in training. Perhaps it would be the same with dogs
who startle at the feeling of a hand pressed firmly on their
head.
As Temple Grandin has similarly noted with
cows and pigs, causing the meat industry to alter the paths the
animals walk into the slaughterhouse. For the industry, her work is
useful in promoting less stressed, and thus better-tasting meat.
For the animals, they are presumably spared from some added anxiety
as they travel—one hopes unknowingly—toward their deaths.
To pull a dog from ardent sniffing is the same
for him as being yanked away from a scene just as soon as you turn
your eyes to it.
Clicker training tries to address this
dissonance of our different "moments" and our different senses of
what the dog is "doing" at any moment. Trainers use a small device
that allows them to make a sharp, distinct click! when the dog has done a desired behavior and
can expect an imminent reward. The click helps make a human moment
salient to a dog; left to his own devices, the dog parcels up his
life differently.
This might seem a good time for a young dog to
meet his new owner. There is surprisingly little good science about
the timing of this introduction. The forces determining when people
adopt dogs are more often than not influenced by everything
but the best age for a puppy to meet a
person. Many states have laws prohibiting sale of puppies prior to
8 weeks, to protect against selling physically immature animals.
Breeders have their own interests in mind in selling their charges.
But social recognition requires experience. From two weeks to four
months dogs are particularly open to learning about others (of any
species). No dog should be taken away from his mother before he is
weaned (which can be from six to ten weeks), but dogs should be exposed to humans as well as to
littermates.
We are generally enthralled by creatures that
look like us in at least some way. Notably, not every and not all
animals are effused over, taken in, or anthropomorphized: monkeys
and dogs regularly are, but eels and manta rays rarely are. "That
barnacle just loves hanging out with me and my boat" is a sentence
never uttered. The difference between the monkey and the barnacle
is part evolutionary, part familiarity. An infant monkey curling a
hand around a mother's finger easily evokes the same poignant scene
between human mothers and infants. By contrast, however much a
young eel may be yearning for contact as it slides toward its
mother, its lack of limbs gets in the way of our calling the scene
"touching"—or even intentional.
Edward O. Wilson, the naturalist and
sociobiologist who studied ant populations in amazing detail,
proposed that we have an inborn, species-typical tendency to
affiliate with other animals: what has been called the "biophilia
hypothesis." The notion is attractive and also much debated. It is,
notably, difficult to disprove such a hypothesis. Regardless, I
consider it the scientist's way of saying what Woody Allen
did.
In studies with puppies, researchers found
that those distressed at separation from their mothers and
littermates vocalized somewhat less if given a towel or soft toy (a
stuffed blue lamb). If there is knowledge to be gained here it is
that a soft familiar object can be a salve (hence, in children, the
power of teddy bears); in fact, such an object may reduce some of
the unease dogs may manifest at being left at home alone.
*
Neither are these methods benign, in some
cases: there is the famous case of the zebra finches, captured and
harmlessly leg-banded for identification as the researchers
observed their mating tactics. Lo and behold, the only feature that
they found was predictive of a male's success at breeding was the
color of his leg band. Female zebra finches apparently swoon for a
red band on a fella (males prefer black-banded females).