Mute
Pump sits close to me and quietly pants,
gazing at me: she wants something. On our walks she tells me when
we've gone far enough and she is ready to go back: she hops up,
pivots on her rear legs, then beelines back from where we came. I
turn on the bathwater, turn to her with a smile, and her tail drops
and wags low, her ears flattening on her head. All this talking and
yet no talking at all.
There is a certain poignancy in describing
animals as our "dumb friends"; in noting the "blank bewilderment"
of a dog; in nodding at their "uncommunicating muteness." These are
familiar ways of talking about dogs, who never respond in kind as
we speak to them. No small amount of dogs' winsomeness is the
empathy that we can attribute to them as they silently contemplate
us. Still, these characterizations, while evocative, seem to me to
be outright flawed in two ways. First, it is not the animals who
desire to speak and cannot, I suspect; it is that we desire them to
talk and cannot effect it. Second, most animals, and dogs in
particular, are neither blank of expression nor in fact mute. Dogs,
like wolves, communicate with their eyes, ears, tail, and very
posture. Far from pleasantly silent, they squeal, growl, grunt,
yelp, moan, whine, whimper, bark, yawn, and howl. And that's just
in the first few weeks.
Dogs talk. They communicate; they declare;
they express themselves. This comes as no surprise; what is
surprising is how often they are communicating, and in how many
ways. They talk to each other, they talk to you, and they talk to
noises on the other side of closed doors or hidden in high grasses.
This gregariousness is familiar to us: having a large roster of
communications is consistent with being social, as humans are.
Those canids such as foxes, who do not live in a social group,
appear to have a much more limited range of things to say. Even the
kinds of sounds foxes make are indicative of their more solitary
nature: they make sounds that travel well over long distances.
Dogs' staunch unmuteness is expressed through making sounds
bellowed and whispered. Vocalizations, scent, stance, and facial
expression each function to communicate to other dogs and, if we
know how to listen, to us.
OUT
LOUD
Two human beings stroll through a park chatting. They move with ease from commenting on the warmth of the air, to the nature of humans in positions of power, to expressions of mutual adoration, to reflections on past expressions of mutual adoration, to admonishment to observe the tree straight ahead. They do this primarily by making small, strange contortions of the shape of the cavities of their mouths, the placement of their tongues, by pushing air through the vocal tract and squeezing or widening their lips. Theirs is not the only communication going on. Over the course of a walk, the dogs by their sides may scold one another, confirm friendships, court each other, declare dominance, rebuff advances, claim ownership of a stick, or assert allegiance to their person. Dogs, like so many non-human animals, have evolved innumerable, non-language-driven methods to communicate with one another. Human facility at communication is unquestionable. We converse with an elaborate, symbol-driven language, quite unlike anything seen in other animals. But we sometimes forget that even non-language-using creatures might be talking up a storm.
What animals have are whole systems of
behavior that get information from a sender (speaker) to a
recipient (listener). That is all that is needed to call something
a communication. It needn't be important, relevant, or even
interesting information, but between animals it often is.
Communication is only sometimes within our range of hearing, or
even vocal: it is often made through body language—using limbs,
head, eyes, tails, or the entire body—or even through such
surprising forms as changing color, urinating and defecating, or
making oneself larger or smaller.
We can spot a communication by noticing if,
after one animal makes a noise or does an action, another responds
to it by changing its behavior. Information has been imparted. What
we'll miss, since we don't know the language of, say, spiders or
sloths (though there are currently researchers trying to learn
these communication systems), is those utterances that fall on deaf
ears. Still, animals are constant gabbers. The discoveries of
natural science over the last one hundred years have shown us the
variety of guises in which this gabbing can appear. Birds twitter,
peep, and sing songs—so do humpback whales. Bats emit
high-frequency clicks; elephants, low-frequency rumbles. The
wiggling dance of a honeybee communicates the direction, quality,
and distance to food; a monkey's yawn conveys a threat. A firefly's
flashes indicate his species; a poison-dart frog's coloration
identifies his toxicity.
The kind we notice first is the one that most
closely matches our own language: communication out loud.
DOG-EARED
Thunder outside. Pump's ears, velvet
equilateral triangles that fold perfectly along the side of her
head, prick into long isosceles. Head up, eyes to the window, she
identifies the sound: a storm, a frightful thing. Her ears pivot
back, flattened along her skull as if to hold them shut by their
own force. I coo to her consolingly and watch her ears for
feedback. The tips soften but she relaxes only slightly, still
holding them tight against the roar.
Without prominent ears ourselves, we can envy dogs' proud ears. They come in a dazzling array of equally adorable variants: extremely long and lobular; small, soft, and perked; folding gracefully alongside the face. Dogs' ears may be mobile or rigid, triangular or rounded, floppy or upright. In most dogs, the pinna—the outer, visible part of the ear—rotates to better open a channel from the sound source to the inner ear. The practice of cropping ears, severing the pinnae to make floppy ears stand upright, long mandated in many breed standards, is becoming less popular. This designing of dogs, sometimes defended as reducing infections, has unknown consequences in auditory sensitivity.
By natural design, dogs' ears have evolved to
hear certain kinds of sounds. Happily, that set of sounds overlaps
with those we can hear and produce: if we utter it, it will at
least hit the eardrum of a nearby dog. Our auditory range is from
20 hertz to 20 kilohertz: from the lowest pitch on the longest
organ pipe to an impossibly squeaky squeak.* We spend most of our
time straining to understand sounds between 100 hertz and 1
kilohertz, the range of any interesting speech going on in the
vicinity. Dogs hear most of what we hear and then some. They can
detect sounds up to 45 kilohertz, much higher than the hair cells
of our ears bother to bend to. Hence the power of the dog whistle,
a seemingly magical device that makes no apparent sound and yet
perks the ears of dogs for blocks around. We call this sound
"ultrasonic," since it's beyond our ken, but it is within the sonic
range for many animals in our local environment. Don't think for a
moment that apart from the occasional dog whistle, the world is
quiet for dogs up at those high registers. Even a typical room is
pulsing with high frequencies, detectable by dogs constantly. Think
your bedroom is quiet when you rise in the morning? The crystal
resonator used in digital alarm clocks emits a never-ending alarm
of high-frequency pulses audible to canine ears. Dogs can hear the
navigational chirping of rats behind your walls and the bodily
vibrations of termites within your walls. That compact fluorescent
light you installed to save energy? You may not hear the hum, but
your dog probably can.
The range of pitches we are most intent on are
those used in speech. Dogs hear all sounds of speech, and are
nearly as good as we are at detecting a change of pitch—relevant,
say, for understanding statements, which end in a low pitch, versus
questions, which in English end in a raised pitch: "Do you want to
go for a walk(?)" With the question mark, this sentence is exciting
to a dog with experience going on walks with humans. Without it, it
is simply noise. Imagine the confusion generated by the recent
growth of "up-talking," speech that ends every sentence with the
sound of a question?
If dogs understand the stress and tones—the
prosody—of speech, does this hint that
they understand language? This is a natural but vexed question.
Since language use is one of the most glaring differences between
the human animal and all other animals, it has been proposed as the
ultimate, incomparable criterion for intelligence. This raises
serious hackles in some animal researchers (not thought of as a
hackled species, ironically), who have set about trying to
demonstrate what linguistic ability animals have. Even those
researchers who may agree that language is necessary for
intelligence have nonetheless added reams of results to the growing
pile of evidence of linguistic ability in non-human animals. All
parties agree, though, that there has been no discovery of a
humanlike language—a corpus of infinitely combinable words that
often carry many definitions, with rules for combining words into
meaningful sentences—in animals.
This is not to say that animals might not
understand some of our language use, even if they don't produce it
themselves. There are, for instance, many examples of animals
taking advantage of the communicative system of nearby unrelated
animal species. Monkeys can make use of nearby birds' warning calls
of a nearby predator to themselves take protective action. Even an
animal who deceives another animal by mimicry—which some snakes,
moths, and even flies can do—is in some way using another species's
language.
The research with dogs suggests that they do
understand language—to a limited degree. On the one hand, to say
that dogs understand words is a
misnomer. Words exist in a language, which itself is product of a
culture; dogs are participants in that culture on a very different
level. Their framework for understanding the application of the
word is entirely different. There is, no doubt, more to the words
of their world than Gary Larson's Far
Side comics suggest: eat, walk, and fetch. But he is on to
something, insofar as these are organizing elements of their
interaction with us: we circumscribe the dog's world to a small set
of activities. Working dogs seem miraculously responsive and
focused compared to city pets. It is not that they are innately
more responsive or focused, but that their owners have added to
their vocabularies types of things to do.
One component in understanding a word is the
ability to discriminate it from other words. Given their
sensitivity to the prosody of speech, dogs do not always excel at
this. Try asking your dog on one morning to go
for a walk; on the next, ask if your dog wants to
snow forty locks in the same voice. If
everything else remains the same, you'll probably get the same,
affirmative reaction. The very first sounds of an utterance seem to
be important to dog perception, though, so changing the swallowed
consonants for articulated ones and the long vowels for short
ones—ma for a
polk?—might prompt the confusion merited by this gibberish.
Of course humans read meaning into prosody, too. English does not
give the prosody of speech syntactical leverage but it is still
part of how we interpret "what has just been said."
If we were more sensitive to the sound of what we say to dogs, we might get better
responses from them. High-pitched sounds mean something different
than low sounds; rising sounds contrast with falling sounds. It is
not accidental that we find ourselves cooing to an infant in silly,
giddy tones (called motherese)—and
might greet a wagging dog with similar baby talk. Infants can hear
other speech sounds, but they are more interested in motherese.
Dogs, too, respond with alacrity to baby talk—partially because it
distinguishes speech that is directed at them from the rest of the continuous yammering
above their heads. Moreover, they will come more easily to
high-pitched and repeated call requests than to those at a lower
pitch. What is the ecology behind this? High-pitched sounds are
naturally interesting to dogs: they might indicate the excitement
of a tussle or the shrieking of nearby injured prey. If a dog fails
to respond to your reasonable suggestion that he come right now, resist the urge to lower and sharpen
your tone. It indicates your frame of mind—and the punishment that
might ensue for his prior uncooperativeness. Correspondingly, it is
easier to get a dog to sit on command
to a longer, descending tone rather than repeated, rising notes.
Such a tone might be more likely to induce relaxation, or
preparation for the next command from their talky human.
There is one celebrated dog whose word usage
is exceptional. Rico, a border collie in Germany, can identify over
two hundred toys by name. Given an enormous heap of all the toys
and balls he has ever seen, he can reliably pull out and retrieve
the one his owner requests. Now, putting aside why a dog might need
two hundred toys, this ability is impressive. Children are
hard-pressed to do the same task (and are only sometimes helpful in
bringing things back). Even better, Rico can quickly learn a name
for a new object, by process of elimination. Experimenters put a
novel toy among familiar ones and asked him, using a word he had
never heard before, to retrieve it. Go get the
snark, Rico. One would be sympathetic if he looked bemused,
and wandered back with a favorite toy in his chops. Instead,
though, Rico reliably picked out the new toy: naming it.
Rico was not using language, of course, in the
way we, or even young children, do. One can debate how much he was
understanding, or if he was even doing
anything other than showing a preference for the new object. On the
other hand, he was showing an astute ability to satisfy the humans
making various sounds by picking up the referents of those sounds.
His ability might not indicate that all dogs are so able: Rico
might be an unusually skilled word user*
—and he is definitely unusually motivated by
the praise received on retrieving the right toy. Still, even if he
were the only dog who does this, it indicates that the dog's
cognitive equipment is good enough to understand language in the
right context.
It is not only the express content or sound of
speech that carries meaning. Being a competent language user means
understanding the pragmatics of usage: how the means, form, and
context of what you say also affect the meaning of what you say.
Paul Grice, a twentieth-century philosopher, famously described
various "conversational maxims," known to us implicitly, that
regulate language use. Their use marks you as a cooperative
speaker; even their express violation is often meaningful. They
include the charming maxim of relation (be relevant), the maxim of
manner (be brief and clear), and maxims of quality (tell the truth)
and quantity (say only as much as you need to).
On a good day, dogs mind all of Grice's
maxims. Consider a dog who espies a roguish-looking fellow down the
street. The dog may bark (relevant: the guy is roguish-looking)
sharply (quite unambiguous), but only as long as the fellow is
around (so the warning bark is currently true), and not more than a
few times (relatively pithy). While dogs hardly qualify as
competent language users, it is notably not because of their
violation of the pragmatics of communication. It is only the
smallness of their vocabulary and restricted use of words in
combination that disqualifies them.
Many owners lament that, by contrast to Rico,
their dogs are not terrific listeners—despite their broad range of
audition. To be fair, canids do not rely on hearing as their
primary sense. Relative to even our hearing, their ability to
pinpoint where a sound is coming from is imprecise. They hear
sounds unmoored from their origins. And just like us, they must
bring attention to a noise to hear it best—first apparent in the
familiar tilt of the head, to direct the ears slightly toward the
sound source, or in radar-dish adjustments of the pinnae. Instead
of being used to "see" the source of the sound, their auditory
sense seems to serve an ancillary function: helping dogs find the
general direction of a sound, at which point they can turn on a
more acute sense, like olfaction or even vision, to investigate
further.
Dogs themselves make a variety of sounds
across a range of pitches or differing only by subtle alterations
in tempo or frequency. They are downright noisy.
THE OPPOSITE OF MUTE
Her slow, light panting, mouth open partway,
tongue purple and wet and perfect. Pump's panting was a
conversation in and of itself—I always felt talked-to when she
panted at me.
The cacophony of a packed dog run seems at
first pass to be an undifferentiated racket. With closer attention,
though, one can distinguish shouts from cries; yelps from barks;
and play barks from threatening barks. Dogs make sounds both
intentionally and inadvertently. Both kinds may hold information,
the minimum requirement to call an aural disturbance a
"communication" rather than simply "noise." What is interesting for
scientists is determining the meaning of that information. Given
the way dogs wield these noises, there is no doubt that they have
different meanings.
Countless hours of researchers' lives spent
listening to animals shout, coo, click, groan, and scream has led
to the discovery of some universal features of sound signals. They
either express something about the world—a discovery, a danger—or
something about the signalers themselves—their identity, sexual
status, rank, membership in a group, fear, or pleasure. They effect
a change in others: they may decrease social distance between the
signaler and those around him, calling someone closer; or increase
social distance, frightening someone away. In addition, sounds may
serve to cohere a group (in defense from a predator or intruder,
for instance) or they may elicit maternal or sexual affiliation.
Ultimately, all these purposes for making sounds make evolutionary
sense: they aid the animal in securing its survival or the survival
of its relatives.
What, then, are dogs saying, and how are they
saying it? The what is answered by
looking at the context of making a sound. The context includes not
just the sounds around it but also the means: a screamed word winds
up meaning something different than one intoned with a sultry
whisper. A sound a dog makes while wagging merrily means something
different than the same sound delivered through bared
teeth.
The meaning of an uttered sound can also be
identified by looking at what those who hear it do. Although human
responses to an utterance (say, How are
you?) may range from the appropriate (I'm well, thanks) to the seeming non sequitur
(Yes, we have no
bananas), there is reason to believe that dogs, and all
non-human animals, respond ingenuously. In many cases, a sound will
have a reliable effect on those in the vicinity: think Fire! or Free
money!
The how of sound
signaling is simple with dogs. Most of the sounds dogs make are
oral: using or coming out of the mouth. At least, these are the
sounds that we know about. These vocal sounds might be voiced, with
vibration in the larynx—the airway used for breathing—or may be
expiratory—part of an exhalation. Others are entirely unvoiced but
use the mouth, such as the mechanical sound of tooth-snapping.
Vocal sounds vary from one another along four easily audible
dimensions. They vary in pitch (frequency): whines are nearly
always high-pitched, while growls are low-pitched. Try and squeal
out a growl and it becomes something else. They vary in duration:
some are uttered once, quickly, lasting less than half a second;
others are protracted sounds or are repeated again and again.
Sounds vary in their shape: some are pure tones while others are
more fractured, fluctuating or rising and falling. A howl has
little variation for long periods, while barks are noisy,
changeable sounds. Finally, they vary in loudness or intensity.
Moans don't come in loud and yelps don't come in a
whisper.
WHIMPERS, GROWLS, SQUEAKS, AND CHUCKLES
She sees I'm almost ready. With her head fixed
on the ground between her paws, Pump follows me with her eyes as I
cross the room gathering my bag, a book, my keys. I scratch her
around her ears in consolation and break for the door. She lifts
her head and makes a sound: a plaintive yelp. I freeze. A look back
and she hurries over, wagging. Okay, then; I guess she'll come with
me.
The paradigmatic dog sound is the bark, but
barks do not form the preponderance of most dogs' daily
noisemaking, which includes high and low sounds, incidental sounds,
even howls and chuckles. High-frequency sounds—cries, squeals,
whines, whimpers, yelps, and screams—occur when the dog is in
sudden pain or needs attention. These are some of the first sounds
a puppy will make, which clues us in to their meaning: they tend to
attract attention of the mother. A yelp
might come out of a puppy who was just stepped on, or who has
wandered off. Deaf and blind, it is easier for mom to find her pup
than vice versa. Having been reunited, some continue to yelp,
winding down off their crying jag, when carried by their mothers.
Yelps are different than screams, which
in wolves prompt the mother to groom the pup, providing the contact
that is necessary for normal development. Cries and squeals may
be ignored by the mother, and so a particular squeal may be a less
specifically meaningful utterance and instead a general-purpose
sound used simply to see how others respond.
Low moans or
grunts are also very common in puppies,
and seem not to be signs of pain but rather a kind of dog purr.
There are snuffling moans and sighing moans—what some call
"contentment grunts," and they all seem to mean about the same
thing. Pups moan when they are in close contact with littermates,
their mother, or a well-known human caretaker. The sound might be
simply a result of heavy, slow breathing, which indicates it might
not be intentionally produced: there is no evidence that dogs moan
on purpose (neither is there evidence that they do not; neither has
been proven). But whether they do or not, moans probably function
to affirm the bond between family members, whether heard as a low
vibration or felt through skin-to-skin contact.
The rumble of a growl and the steady ominous snarl, you won't need to be told, are aggressive
sounds. Puppies do not tend to produce them, as puppies do not tend
to initiate aggression. Part of what makes them aggressive is their
low pitch: they are the kind of sounds that would come out of a
large animal, rather than the high-pitched squeals of a small one.
In an antagonistic (what in biology is called agonistic) encounter with another animal, a dog
wants to appear to be the bigger, more powerful creature—so he
makes a big-dog sound. By making higher-pitched sounds, an animal
sounds, simply, smaller: a friendly or appeasing noise, by
contrast. Though aggressive in intent, growls are still
social sounds, not just utterances
produced when a dog feels fear or anger: for the most part, dogs do
not growl at inanimate objects,* or even at animate objects that
aren't faced or directed toward them. They are also subtler than we
think: distinct growls, from rumble to nearly roaring, are used in
different contexts. The growl of tug-of-war may sound fearsome, but
it is nothing like the possessive warning snarled over a treasured
bone. Play these growls back over a speaker set up right in front
of a desirable bone and dogs in the vicinity will avoid the
bone—even with no dog in sight. But if the speaker growls only play
or stranger growls, nearby dogs go ahead and grab the unguarded
bone.
Incidental sounds of dogs are sometimes
produced so reliably in certain contexts that they have become
effectively communicative. The play
slap, an audible landing on the two forefeet at once, is an
inevitable part of play. It conveys sufficient exuberance that it
can be used by itself to ask a dog to play with you. Some dogs
chatter their teeth in anxious
excitement, and the clicking of teeth serves as a warning that the
dog is wary. An exaggerated shriek on
being nosed or bitten roughly in play can even become a ritualized
deception, a way to get out of a social interaction that is making
the dog uncertain. The snuffling sound
created when reaching the head vertically up and sniffing for food
around a human mouth can become not just a search for food but also
a request for food. Even the noisy breathing created by lying so
close as to have the nose pressed against another body comes to
indicate a state of contented relaxation.
If you live with a hound, you are familiar
with the howl. From a staccato baying
to a mournful wail, howling in dogs seems to be a behavior left
over from their ancestors, living in social packs. Wolves howl when
separated from the group, and also when setting out with the group
for a hunt or in reunion afterward. A howl when alone is a
communication seeking company; howling together may be simply a
rallying cry or celebration of the group. It has a contagious
component, leading others in the vicinity to pick it up in an
impromptu fugue. We do not know what they are saying, to each other
or to the moon.
The most social of human sounds is the
cackling laugh rumbling across the room. Do dogs laugh? Well, only
when something is terrific fun. Yes, dogs have what has been called
a laugh. It is not identical to human laughter, the spontaneous
sounds spit out in response to something funny, surprising, or even
frightening. Nor is it as variable as the cackles, giggles, and
twitters that we produce. The dog laugh is a breathy exhalation
that sounds like an excited burst of panting. We could call it
social panting: it is a pant only heard
when dogs are playing or trying to get someone to play with them.
Dogs don't seem to laugh to themselves, off sitting in the corner
of the room, recollecting how that tawny dog in the park outsmarted
her human this morning. Instead, dogs laugh when interacting
socially. If you have played with your dog, you have probably heard
it. In fact, doing your own social panting toward a dog is one of
the most effective ways to elicit play.
Just as our laughs are often inadvertent,
reflexive responses, so may dog laughs be: simply the kind of
panting that results when you're throwing your body around in play.
Though it might not be under the control of the dog, social panting
does seem to be a sign of enjoyment. And it may induce enjoyment—or
at least alleviate stress—in others: playing a recording of the
sounds of dog laughter at animal shelters has been found to reduce
barking, pacing, and other signs of stress in the dogs housed
there. Whether mirth feels like what it does in humans is yet to be
studied.
WOOF
I can remember the first time a bark came out
of Pump, when she was maybe three years old. She'd been so quiet
until then, and then one day, after spending time with her barky
German shepherd friend, a bark popped out of her. It was bark
like more than a bark, as though a
sound that stood for a bark but wasn't itself the real thing: a
well-articulated rurph! accompanied by
a little leap off her front legs and a madly wagging tail. She
refined this splendid display somewhat through the years, but it
always felt like a new dog thing she was trying on.
It is regrettable that barks tend to be such loud affairs. The bark is shouted. While a calm conversation between the two strollers in the park might register about 60 decibels, dog barks begin at 70 decibels and a stream of barks may be punctuated with spikes to 130 decibels. Increases in decibels, the unit of measurement of the loudness of sounds, are exponential: an increase of 10 decibels describes a hundredfold rise in the experience of the strength of a sound. One hundred and thirty decibels is up there with thunderclaps and plane takeoffs. The bark is momentary, but it is a moment of displeasure for our ears. The reason this is regrettable is that there is, most dog researchers agree, much information in those barks. Given the relative scarcity of barking in wolves, some theorize that dogs have developed a more elaborate barking language precisely in order to communicate with humans. If we consider barks as all cut from the same cloth, though, they are likelier to annoy than to communicate.
Researchers might not call barks "annoying,"
but they call them "chaotic" and "noisy." "Chaotic" is a good
description for the variability in the kinds of sounds within each
bark; "noisy" means not just disagreeably
loud but also having
fluctuations in its structure. Barks
are loud, and different barks have varying numbers of harmonic
components, depending on the context in which the bark is
used.
Still, of the sounds dogs make, barks come
closest to speech sounds. The dog's bark is, like the phonemes of
speech, produced by vibrations in the vocal folds and air flowing
along the folds and through the mouth cavity. Perhaps because they
are in overlapping frequencies—from 10 hertz to 2 kilohertz—with
the sounds of speech, we are inclined to look for speechlike
meaning in them. We even name the bark using phonemes from our
language: the dog "woofs," "rufs," "arfs," or (although no dog I
know says it) "bow-wows." The French hear the dog "ouah-ouah";
Norwegian dogs voff-voff; Italian dogs
go bau-bau.
Some ethologists think that barking is not
fundamentally communicating anything, though: that it is
"ambiguous" and "meaningless." This view is encouraged by the
difficulty of deciphering what the meaning of the barks could be,
since sometimes dogs bark without an obvious prompt or audience, or
continue to bark long after any message therein would have been
conveyed. Think of the dog barking continuously, dozens of times in
a row, in front of another dog: If there is meaning in that bark,
would not one or two repetitions do to convey it?
This strikes at the heart of the trouble in
determining the subjective experience of an animal of which you
cannot ask questions. Each moment of an animal's behavior is
scrutinized for its meaning. Surely few human actions could bear
such scrutiny and yield a correct assessment of the human. If you
were to videotape me practicing, at home in front of my dog, a
speech I am to deliver later in the day, you might well conclude
that (a) I believe the dog can understand what I'm saying,
or
(b) I am talking to myself. In either case,
(c) holds: the noises I am making appear not to be classically
communicative—for I do not have an audience who can understand me.
Similarly, examples of poor communication by a dog may seem to
undermine the notion that dogs communicate at all. But most
researchers think that barks do have meaning, albeit one dependent
on the context and even on the individual. Barking, especially
alarm barking, is one of the clearest distinctions between dogs and
other canid species. Wolves bark to convey alarm, but rarely, and
they make more of a "woof" sound than anything like the protracted
dog barking with which we are familiar. Dogs do not just bark more
than wolves; they have developed numerous variations on the
theme.
There are a handful of distinguishable barks,
used reliably in a handful of distinguishable cases. Dogs bark to
get attention, to warn of danger, in fear, as a greeting, in play,
or even out of loneliness, anxiety, confusion, distress, or
discomfort. The meaning is in the context of their use, but not
only in the context: spectrograms of dog barks show that they are
mixtures of the tones used in growls, in whimpers, and in yelps. By
altering the prevalence of one tone over the others, the bark takes
on a different character—a different gist.
Early research into dogs' vocalizations
concluded that all dog barking was attention-getting barking. In
fact they do attract attention, assuming someone is close enough to
hear them. But recent studies have made more subtle discriminations
between barks. While in some way all barks come down to some manner
of "attention-getting," one might as well say that we speak in
order to be heard: true, but incomplete. For instance, when
experimenters analyzed the spectrograms of thousands of dog barks
during one of three contexts—a stranger ringing the doorbell, being
locked outside, or in play—they found three distinct types of
barks.
Stranger barks
were the lowest in pitch and the harshest: they are nearly spat
out. Less variable than the other types, stranger barks are well
designed to send a message over a distance, something necessary if
caught in a threatening situation alone. They can also be combined
into "superbarks," concatenations of barks that together last much
longer than the duration of barking in other contexts. The end
result is a bark that most human listeners find to be
aggressive.
The isolation
barks tended to be higher-frequency and more variable: some
ranged from loud to soft and back again, some went from high to
low. These barks are lobbed into the air one by one, sometimes with
great intervals between them. They sound "fearful," people tend to
say.
Play barks, too,
are high-frequency, but they happen more often one after the other
than the isolation barks. They're directed at someone else, unlike
isolation barks: at a dog or human playmate. There is considerable
individual variation, of course: not every dog barks alike. The
stranger bark of a small dog may come out as rar, rar or raoaw,
raoaw, while a larger dog emits a capital-r Rumph.
These differences between bark types make
evolutionary sense: the lowest sounds are used in threatening
situations (again to appear bigger); higher sounds are
entreaties—to friends, for companionship—and as such are submissive
requests, not warnings. Differences between individual barkers
indicate that barks might be used to affirm a dog's identity, or
reveal an association with a group (even the group me and the woman at the end of
my leash, rather than these dogs I'm
frolicking among). And barking together with others may be a
form of social cohesion. Barking can be contagious, like the howl:
one dog barking might prompt a chorus of barking dogs, all joined
in their shared noisiness.
BODY AND TAIL
When we approached people on the street, Pump set all her senses to looking; if she recognized them, her head would lower ever so slightly—looking up coyly as though over reading glasses—and she would wag her tail low. This was quite different from her approach of a dog she was smitten with: all upright, tail high, posture impeccable, wags soldierlike in their rhythm—or a dog friend—a looser,
janglier approach, and even an open-mouthed grab toward their face, or a gentle bump with her hip along their body.
You may be sitting down right now, folded into
a comfy chair—or perhaps you're standing, straphanging on a train
to work, book scrunched against another commuter's back. Most
likely you don't mean anything by your
sitting or standing, or when you walk or lie supine: it's just a
posture of convenience or comfort. But in other contexts our very
posture conveys information. A catcher crouches: he's prepared for
a pitch. A parent crouches and opens his arms: he's inviting a
child for a hug. Running when someone you know approaches, you
suddenly stop and greet them; standing still when someone you know
approaches, you suddenly turn and run. There can be meaning simply
in the vigor or slouch of your body. For an animal with a limited
vocal repertoire, posture is ever more important. And it appears
that dogs use specific postures to make very specific
statements.
There is a language of the body, formed of
phonemes made from rumps, heads, ears, legs, and tails. Dogs know
how to translate this language intuitively; I learned it after
watching hundreds of hours of dogs interacting with each other. We
must look like such stiffs to dogs, who can express everything from
playfulness to aggression to amorous intent by changing the shape
of their body and its altitude. By contrast, we are inhibited
straight-backeds, mostly stationary or traveling forward with
little excess movement. Occasionally—heavens—we turn a head or arm
flamboyantly to the side.
But man himself cannot express love and
humility by external signs, so plainly as does a dog, when with
drooping ears, hanging lips, flexuous body, and wagging tail, he
meets his beloved master.—Charles Darwin
For dogs, posture can announce aggressive
intent or shrinking modesty. To simply stand erect, at full height,
with head and ears up, is to announce readiness to engage, and
perhaps to be the prime mover in the engagement. Even the hair
between the shoulders or at the rump—the hackles—may be standing at attention, serving not
just as a visual signal of arousal but also releasing the odor of
the skin glands at the base of the hairs. To exaggerate the whole
effect, a dog might stand not just up but over another dog, head or paws on his back. That's
about as declarative a statement as you can make that you are
feeling dominant. The opposite body posture, crouching with head
down, ears down, and tail tucked away, is submissive. To lie all
the way down and expose the belly is even more so.*
This principle of antithesis—that opposing postures communicate
opposing emotions—describes much of the expressive scope of dogs.
Facial expressions, most visible in the mouth and ears, mind this
principle, too. The mouth sweeps from closed to open and relaxed,
to open with lips raised, snout wrinkled, and teeth bared. A dog's
"grin," with jaw closed, is submissive; as the mouth is opened, the
arousal increases; and if the teeth are exposed, the look gets
aggressive. Coming full circle, a wide-open mouth with teeth mostly
covered—a yawn—is not a sign of boredom, as often assumed by
analogy with our own yawning; instead, it may indicate anxiety,
timidity, or stress, and is used by dogs to calm themselves or
others. The ears can also go through these gymnastics: they can be
pricked, relaxed and down, or folded tightly along the head.
Eyeballing another dog directly can be threatening or aggressive;
by contrast, looking away is submissive—an attempt to quell one's
own anxiety and the other dog's arousal. In other words, in each
case there is a range from one extreme to another, representing
variation in intensity along an emotional continuum, from relaxed
to aroused in fear or alarm.
None of these is a static symbol—or, if it is,
its being static is meaningful. Holding an erect posture,
motionless, is a quiet way of putting an exclamation point on the
posture. It exaggerates the tenseness of the communication. For the
most part, postures are taken, and moved through. The tail,
especially, is a limb of movement. It is to science's great
discredit that no one has done a thorough investigation of the
meaning of every wag of the dog's tail.
As a puppy her tail was trim, an arrow of soft
black fur. This turned out not to be the destiny of her tail at
all: it grew into an incredible banner of a tail, with extravagant
feathering that matted and gathered leaves. It was bent at the tip
from a disagreement with a car door when she was young. She
brandished it when excited or delighted, curved to a sickle with
the tip pointing at her back. When lying down, she drummed it
happily on the ground at my approach. Her tail registered her
exhaustion in a low-hanging straight pose; her disinterest in a
nosy dog by tucking between her legs. Most of the time as we walked
together, it hung loosely down, curving jauntily toward its tip,
and merrily swished to and fro. I loved to approach her slowly,
stalking her, and prompt her tail to quiver into wagging.
One of the difficulties in deciphering the language of tails is the great variation in tails among dogs. The flamboyant plumage of a golden retriever contrasts mightily with the tight corkscrew of the pug. Dogs wear tails long and rigid, stumpy and curled, hanging heavily or perpetually perked. The wolf tail is in some ways an average of the various breed tails: it is a long, slightly feathered tail, held naturally slightly down. Early ethologists who did a reckoning of wolf tail postures identified at least thirteen different tail carriages, conveying thirteen distinguishable messages. As per the antithesis thesis, tails held high indicate confidence, self-assertion, or excitement from interest or aggression, while low-hanging tails indicate depression, stress, or anxiety. An erect tail also exposes the anal region, allowing a bold dog to air his odor signature. By contrast, a long tail held so low as to curl back between the legs, closing off the rump, is actively submissive and fearful. When a dog is simply waiting around, his tail is relaxed, hanging low, dropped down but not rigid. A tail gently lifted is a sign of mild interest or alertness.
But it is not as simple as tail height, for
the tail is not just held, it is wagged. Wagging does not translate
as simple happiness. A high, stiffly wagging tail can be a threat,
especially when accompanied by an erect posture. Quickly wagging a
dropped, low tail is another sign of submission. This is the tail
of the dog who has just been caught finishing off the last of your
shoes. The vigor of the wag is roughly indicative of the intensity
of the emotions. A neutral tail wagging lightly is interested but
tentative. A loose, lively whisking tail accompanies the noseled
search for a ball lost in high grasses or an odor trail discovered
on the ground. The familiar happy wag is incredibly different from
all of these: the tail is held above or out from the body and
strongly draws rough arcs in the air behind it. Unmistakable
delight. Even non-wagging is meaningful: dogs tend to still their
tails when attending carefully to a ball in your hand or waiting
for you to tell them what's happening next.
Researchers interested in the brain of the dog
accidentally discovered something about the tail of the dog along
the way: the dog wags asymmetrically. On average, dogs' wags tend
more strongly to the right when they suddenly see their owners—or
even anything else of some interest: another person, or a cat. When
presented with an unfamiliar dog, dogs still wag—more that
tentative wag than the happy wag—but tending to the left. You might
not be able to see this in your own dog unless you watch them wag
in slow-motion video playback (which I highly recommend)—or unless
your dog is one of those who wag less back-and-forth than
round-and-round, inclining to the side. Consider yourself lucky to
be wagged at with such clear-cut enthusiasm.
Pump does a full-body shake: it starts in her
head and rolls down her body, shimmering out through her tail. It
is like a punctuation mark that has yet to be discovered. She
shakes to end an episode, when she's unsure, and sometimes when
she's just ambling along.
The dog uses his body expressively:
communication writ through movement. Even the moments between
interactions are marked by movement: as when a dog does a full-body
shake, his skin twisting over his frame, to indicate his finishing
one activity and moving on to another. Not all dogs have hackles
that visibly raise with pique, long tails to ostentatiously wag, or
ears that raise with interest. The fabulously ropy-furred komondor
approaches other dogs with what we must assume is his head, but
neither eyes nor ears are visible underneath his long locks. In
breeding dogs to have particular looks that we find agreeable, we
are limiting their possibilities for communicating. Just as we
might expect, but would rather not confront, a dog with a docked
tail has, thereby, a docked repertoire of things he can
say.
Research looking at the range and rate of
signals used by ten physically dissimilar breeds found just this.
Comparing the behavior of dogs from the Cavalier King Charles
spaniel to the French bulldog to the Siberian husky, there was a
clear relationship between the breed appearance and the number of
signals they used. Those animals that had been most changed
physically in domestication from wolves—the King Charles, at the
extreme—sent the fewest signals. These pedomorphic or neotenous dogs, who retain more features of
juvenile members of canid species into adulthood, look least like
adult wolves. The huskies, which have the most wolflike features
and are genetically closer to Canis
lupus, do the most wolflike signals.
Given that many bodily signals provide
information about one's status, strength, or intent, the necessity
for dogs to send these signals is presumably diminished in a world
where humans chaperone dogs through life. But the same signals used
to convince a dominant animal of benign intent may also be used to
communicate information to humans. Walking through the city, I turn
a blind corner and nearly step on an unfamiliar dog pulling on a
long leash. Seeing me, she crouches, wags her tail furiously
between her legs, and licks toward my face. It may have begun as a
submissive gesture, but now it is adorable.
INADVERTENT AND INTENT
After sleeping late and enduring the slow pace
of my morning rituals, Pump's first move when we get outside never
varies. She takes two steps out the door and unceremoniously
squats. She crouches deeply, fully committed to the pose, with only
her tail—curled high out of the way—pulling her body up. The
torrent of urine released (surely record-breaking this time) seems
accompanied by a relaxing of the muscles of her face—and by a
growing guilt on my own that I made her wait so long. She watches
the stream of her own urine wandering by her as it finds cracks in
the sidewalk in which to divert itself cleanly.
As much as is being said with a bark, snarl,
or wagging tail, vocalization and posture are not the only media of
communication for dogs. Neither is a match for the informational
possibilities of smells. Urination, as we saw earlier, is the means
of odorant communication most conspicuous to us. It might be hard
to believe that the release of the bladder is a "communicative act"
right up there with a polite conversation between friends or a
politician orating before his constituents. At some level, it is
like both of these: it is part of normal dog sociality, and it can
also be a bellowing self-promotion writ on a hydrant.
You might balk at calling the moist message
left high on a lowly fire hydrant the same kind of communication
that humans use—and not just because they are talking out of their
rumps, not their faces. Crucially, we communicate (most of the
time) with intention: rather than
yammering out loud to our own left hand, we tend to direct our
communications to other people—people who are near enough to hear
us, not otherwise distracted, who know the language, and can
understand what we're saying. Intention distinguishes communication
done with others in mind from the automatic oof! uttered on being hit in the belly, blushing at
a compliment, a mosquito's constant buzzing, or the unmindful bit
of information imparting done by traffic lights and flags at
half-mast.
Urine marking is peeing with intention. The
morning's blissful release relieves the strain on the bladder, but
most of the time some is held back for later use in marking.
Presumably the urine is the same urine: there's no evidence of an
independent channel or means by which to modify the odor they are
emitting. But marking behavior differs in a few key ways. First, in
most adult males, and some gender-bending females, marking is
characterized by a prominently raised leg. There are individual and
contextual variations on the so-called "raised-leg display," from a
modest retraction of the rear leg up toward the body, to hoisting
the leg up above the hip points, above vertical—certainly also a
visual display for any other dogs in the vicinity. Both allow for a
directional flow of urine, aimed to land at a conspicuous site.
(One can squat and mark, too, although it is a quieter affair,
perhaps for messages better whispered than shouted.)
Second, the bladder is not emptied when
marking; urine is doled out a little at a time, allowing for
greater distribution of scent over the course of the dog's travels.
If you've left your dog indoors long enough that he races outside
to squat, this urgency might preempt his ability to cache some
urine for later marking. Thus the fruitless raised-leg displays you
may witness, waving dryly at bushes, lampposts, and trash
cans.
Finally, dogs usually urine mark only after
spending some time sniffing the area. This is what elevates the
odor exchange from Lorenz's notion of flag planting to a kind of
conversation. Researchers keeping careful tally of dogs' marking
behavior over time found that who has marked before them, the time
of year, and who is nearby all affect where and when they
mark.
Interestingly, these message bouquets aren't
left indiscriminately: not every surface is marked. Watch a dog
sniff his way down the street: he will sniff more locations than he
will squirt. This indicates that not every message is the same—and
the message this dog will leave may be intended for certain
audiences only. Countermarking—covering old urine with new—is a
common behavior of male dogs, when the old urine is that of less
dominant male dogs. Everyone's marking increases when there is a
new dog around.
If it is not territorial, what is the message
in the mark? The first hint is that puppies don't urine mark: the
communication must have to do with adult concerns. From the
position of the anal glands and the compounds in the urine, we know
that they are at least saying something about who they are: their
odor is their identity. This is a fine message, but it is probably
fairly unintentional. I may communicate something about who I am by
merely walking into a room and being seen, but the very fact of my
person is not a continuous, intentional communication about my
identity (except when I was a kid and dressed to be
seen).
What does look intentional in this
communication is that dogs don't bother to say it if there's no one
else around. Dogs who are kept penned by themselves spend very
little time marking. The males rarely lift a leg to urinate, and
neither sex bothers to deposit just a small amount. Dogs kept in
similar-sized enclosures with other dogs mark much more frequently,
and they mark regularly, every day. The Indian feral dogs marked to
audiences—and audiences of the opposite sex. This makes sense if
the message conveyed is about sex: seeking it oneself, or declaring
oneself fit to be seeked. They did the most raised-leg displays
(even without urinating) when other dogs were present. A leg held
high only gets someone else's attention if someone else is already
there to attend to it.
It also makes sense if the mark is
communication for communication's sake: a comment, an opinion, a
strongly held belief. There is no scientific evidence that it is
so, but it is consistent with communication done only to an
audience. Researchers have found that dogs raised in isolation make
many fewer communicative noises than those raised with other dogs.
When finally around others, though, they begin producing
vocalizations at the same rate that the socialized dogs do. In
other words, they speak when there is someone to speak to.
Just as they mark with intention, so too do
dogs read intention in our markings: in our gestures. As we will
see in the next chapters, they interpret the body language of
humans with the attention they bring to reading each other. As a
young child toddles toward a treasured toy, a dog can see where she
is going and get there first. A turn of the head in thought garners
little attention, but a turn of the head that looks at the
door—there is intention in that turn. And dogs know it. They
realize that there is a difference between gazing toward the door
and turning to look at the clock on the wall; they can distinguish
a finger pointing toward hidden food, and a point done while
lifting one's arm to check a watch. We speak loudly with our
bodies.
A confession: a dog has dictated this entire
chapter to me. She sat by my chair, head on my foot, and patiently
waited while I struggled to translate her words to the page. It is
from her that the insights of the book come, from her that
evocations spring, from her that the scenes and images and umwelt
emerge.
Alas, it is not quite so. But to see the
remarkable number of volumes purportedly written by dogs one must
imagine that this is what we all want: the story straight out of
the dog's mouth—but in our native tongue, of course. At the end of
the nineteenth century, a peculiar kind of autobiography began to
appear in bookshops: it was the "memoir" of your cat, your old dog,
or the animal gone missing in that winter storm. This form,
narrated by talking animals, could be considered the first prose
attempt to get at the point of view of the dog. When I read one of
these—and there are many to choose from, among even such writers as
Rudyard Kipling and Virginia Woolf—a strange discontent washes over
me. It's a sham: there is no perspective of the dog in them.
Instead, it is a dog with the human's voice box transplanted to the
dog's snout. Imagining that dogs' thoughts are but cruder forms of
human discourse does the dog a disservice. And despite their
marvelous range and extent of communication, it is the very fact
that they do not use language that makes me especially treasure
dogs. Their silence can be one of their most endearing traits. Not
muteness: absence of linguistic noise. There is no awkwardness in a
shared silent moment with a dog: a gaze from the dog on the other
side of the room; lying sleepily alongside each other. It is when
language stops that we connect most fully.
Dog-eyed
It takes all of six seconds for Pump to go
from masterful to maladroit. In the first five she flawlessly
navigates the brambles and bushes and thick-trunked trees that web
the opening of the forest into field to catch a fast-moving tennis
ball. It thonks off a tree and she's there to nearly vacuum it into
her mouth. An apparition of a dog tears in out of nowhere, a racing
blur of white fur and bark. Pump notes him and hurtles away,
evading this stealer of tennis balls. In that sixth second, she
stops, suddenly adrift. She's lost track of me. I watch her search:
erect body, head high. I'm within sight; I smile at her. She looks
at and past me, not seeing me. Instead, she spots the large,
limping, heavy-coated man who came through with the white tear. She
takes off after him. I must run to retrieve her. The moment before
Pump was all-seeing; now she's a fool.
There is an intrinsic ranking of the modes by
which we humans sense the world—and vision is winner by a long
shot. Eyes arouse great interest in human psychologists; they
betoken much more than one might imagine just from their physical
form. However pretty a nose one might have, however close the
forehead is to the brain, neither our noses nor foreheads nor
cheeks nor ears are granted such importance.
We are visual animals. There's barely a
challenge for second, either: audition is part of nearly every
experience we have. Olfaction and touch might duke it out for
third, and taste runs a distant fifth. Not that each of these isn't
important to us on any particular occasion. The loveliness of
presentation of, say, a tiered wedding cake would be undercut if
vinegar replaced the anticipated taste of unmitigated sweetness. Or
if any odor besides that of baked goods emanated from the cake—or
if the first bite was not soft and yielding, but crunchy or slimy.
Still, on most occasions we first direct our gaze to a new scene or
object. If we notice something unusual or unexpected on the sleeve
of our jacket, we turn to examine it with our eyes. Vision would
have to really fail to provide any information before we decide to
learn about it by inhaling it closely or taking a bold
lick.
The order of operations is turned upside-down
for dogs. Snout beats eyes and mouth beats ears. Given the
olfactory acuity of dogs, it makes sense that vision plays an
accessory role. When a dog turns his head toward you, it is not so
much to look at you with his eyes; rather, it is to get his nose to
look at you. The eyes just come along for the ride. You may be a
recipient of an imploring gaze by a dog across the room right now.
But can dogs even see what we do?
In many ways, the visual system of a dog—a
subsidiary means to look at the world—is very much like our own.
Its demotion behind other senses, in fact, may allow dogs to see
details with their eyes that we overlook with ours.
One might well ask what a dog would even need
eyes for. They can navigate and find food with their remarkable
noses. Anything that needs closer examination goes right in the
mouth. And they can identify each other through that sensory
apparatus squished between their mouth and nose, the vomeronasal
organ. As it turns out, they have at least two critical uses of
their eyes: to complement their other senses and to see us. The
natural history of the dog eye, seen in the story of their
forebears, wolves, explains the context in which their vision
evolved. It is a happy and transformative side effect that this has
made them good watchers of human beings.
Just one element of the lives of wolves goes a
long way to explaining the eyes they have evolved: eating. Most of
their food runs away. Not only that, it is often camouflaged or
living in the relative safety of herds. It is active—and thus
findable—at dusk, dawn, or night. So wolves, like all predators,
have evolved in response to their prey. As important as scent is,
it cannot serve as the only indicator of the presence of prey, as
air currents send odors on circuitous paths before they reach the
nose. Odors are volatile: if smell lies on a surface, a sensitive
nose can track it specifically; but if it is on the wind, the odor
appears more like a cloud that could have come from one of a
thousand sources. Rapidly moving prey outrun their own odor. Light
waves, by contrast, are transmitted reliably through open air. So
after catching a whiff, wolves use their sight to locate their
prey. Many prey animals are camouflaged to blend in with their
environment. This camouflage is betrayed in motion, however. So
wolves are adept at spotting a change in the visual scene that
indicates that something is moving. Finally, prey animals are often
active at dusk or dawn, a compromise of lighting: easier to hide,
harder to see. In response, wolves developed eyes that are
especially sensitive in low light, and are especially good at
noticing motion in that light.
Her eyes are deep pools of brown and black. It
is hard to see which way they gaze, they are so dark—but it also
makes any glimpse of her irises delightful, as though seeing inside
her soul. Her eyelashes only became apparent when they grayed. Her
eyebrows are also essentially invisible, but the effect of their
moving—as with her head on the floor, to follow me walking across
the room—is visible. In sleep, in dreams, her eyes scanned the
world under their lids. Even closed, the lids reveal a bit of pink
peeking out, as though she were keeping prepared to open her eyes
at once should something important happen nearby.
At first glance these prey-tracking eyes look much like ours: viscous spheres fitted in sockets. Our eyes are about the same size as dogs'. Despite the fact that dogs' heads vary so significantly in size (four Chihuahua heads would fit in a wolfhound's mouth—not that anyone would deign suggest verifying such a thing), eye size barely varies between breeds. Small dogs, like puppies and infants, have large eyes relative to the size of their heads.
But small differences between the eyes of
humans and dogs immediately become apparent. First, our eyes are
smack in the front of our face. We look forward, and images in the
periphery fall away to darkness around our ears. While there is
variation among dogs, most dogs' eyes are situated more laterally
on their heads in the manner of other quadrupeds, allowing a
panoramic view of the environment: 250–270 degrees, as contrasted
to humans' 180 degrees.
If we look a little closer, we discover
another key difference. The superficial anatomy of our eyes gives
us away: it shows where we're looking, how we're feeling, our level
of attention. While dog and human eyes are similarly sized, our
pupil size—the black center of the eye that lets light in—varies
considerably when we are in a darkened room or are aroused or
fearful (expanding to up to 9 millimeters wide) or in the bright
sun or are highly relaxed (contracting to 1 millimeter). Dogs'
pupils, by contrast, are relatively fixed at about 3 to 4
millimeters, regardless of the light or the dog's level of
excitement. Our irises, the muscles that control pupil size, tend
to be colored to contrast with the pupil, blue or brown or green.
Not so in most dogs, whose eyes are often so monochromatically dark
that they remind me of bottomless lakes, repository of all
attributions of purity or desolation we might ascribe to them. And
the human iris sits amidst the sclera—the white—of the eye, while
many (but not all) dogs have very little sclera. The anatomical sum
effect is that we can always see where another person is looking:
the pupil and iris point the way, and the amount of sclera revealed
underlines it. Without a prominent sclera or a distinct pupil, the
eyes of a dog don't indicate the direction of their attention
nearly as much.
Closer still, and we begin to see serious
species differences. Dogs manage to gather more light than we do. Once light enters the eye of
a dog, it travels through the gel-like mass that holds nerve cells
to the retina (we'll get to that in a moment), then through the
retina to a triangle of tissue, which reflects it back. This
tapetum lucidum, in Latin "carpet of light," accounts for
all the photographs you have of your dog with brilliant light
shining out where their eyes should be. Light entering the dog's
eye hits the retina at least twice, resulting not in the redoubling
of the image but in a redoubling of the light that makes images
visible. This is part of the system enabling dogs to have such
improved night and low-light vision. While we might make out a
match being brightly struck in the distance on a dark night, the
dog could detect the gentle flame on the lit candle. Arctic wolves
spend a full half of the year living in utter darkness; if there is
a flame on the horizon, they have the eyes to spot it.
EYES OF THE BALL-HOLDER
It is inside the eye—at that retina receiving light twice—that, one by one, characteristic habits of dogs can be traced to their anatomy. The retina, a sheet of cells on the back of the eyeball, translates light energy into electrical signals to our brains, which leads to our feeling that we've seen something. Much of what we see is given meaning only by our brains, of course—the retina just registers the light—but without the retina, we would experience only darkness. Even slight changes in the conformation of the retina can radically change vision.
There are two slight changes in the canine
retinae: in the distribution of photoreceptor cells and in the
speed with which they operate. The former leads to their ability to
chase prey, retrieve a tossed tennis ball, to their indifference to
most colors, and to their inability to see something right in front
of their noses. The latter leads to their being uninterested in
daytime soap operas left on for them when their owners are out of
the house. We'll look at these in turn.
Go get
the ball!
Some of the most important things for humans
to see are any other humans situated within a few feet of their
face. Our eyes face forward, and our retinae have foveae: central areas with an extra abundance of
photoreceptors. Having so many cells in the center of the retina
means that we are very good at seeing things right in front of us
in high detail, great focus, and strong color. Perfect for
identifying that blob of color and form coming at you as your
boyfriend or your mortal enemy.
Only primates have foveae. By contrast, dogs
have what is called an area
centralis: a broad central region with
fewer receptors than a fovea, but more than in peripheral parts of
the eye. Things directly in front of a dog's face are visible to
him, but they are not quite as sharply in focus as they would be
for us. The lens of the eye, which adjusts its curvature to focus
light onto the retina, doesn't accommodate to nearby sources of
light. In fact, dogs might overlook small things right in front of
their nose (within ten to fifteen inches), because they have fewer
retinal cells committed to receiving light from that part of the
visual world. You need no longer puzzle at your dog's inability to
find the toy that he is nearly stepping on: he's not got the vision
to take note of it until he takes a step back.
Breeds of dogs differ so much in their retinae
that they see the world differently. The area centralis is most
pronounced in those breeds with short noses. Pugs, for instance,
have very strong areas centralis—almost fovea-like. But they lack a
"visual streak," which dogs with long noses (and wolves) have. In
Afghans and retrievers, for instance, the area centralis is less
pronounced, and the photoreceptors of the retina are more dense
along a horizontal band spanning the middle of the eye. The shorter
the nose, the less visual streak; the longer the nose, the more
visual streak. Dogs with the visual streaks have better panoramic,
high-quality vision, and much more peripheral vision than humans.
Dogs with the pronounced area centralis have better focus in front
of their faces.
In a small but significant way, this
difference explains some breed-based behavioral tendencies. Pugs
are not typically so-called "ball dogs" but long-nosed Labrador
retrievers are. Not because of their long noses per se. In addition
to their ability to put their millions of olfactory cells to good
use, Labradors are visually equipped to notice, say, a tennis ball
traveling across the horizon, without having to shift their gaze.
For a short-nosed dog (as for all humans of any nose length), a
ball tossed to the side just disappears into the periphery if they
don't follow it with their head. Instead pugs are probably better
at bringing close objects into focus—say, the faces of their owners
on whose laps they sit. Some researchers speculate that this
relatively blinkered vision makes them more attentive to our
expressions, and seem more companionable.
Go get
the green ball!
Dogs are not color-blind, as is popularly
believed. But color plays a much less important role for them than
it does for humans, and their retinae are why. Humans have three
kinds of cones, the photoreceptors responsible for our perception
of details and of colors: each fires to red, blue, or green
wavelengths. Dogs have only two: one is sensitive to blue and the
other to greenish-yellow. And they have fewer of even those two
than humans do. So dogs experience a color most strongly when it is
in the range of blue or green. Ah, but a well-scrubbed backyard
pool must seem radiant to a dog.
As a result of this difference in cone cells,
any light that looks to us like yellow, red, or orange simply
doesn't look the same way to a dog. Consequently, they seem
perfectly oblivious when you ask them to bring back grapefruits
from the store and grow irritated when they bring back tangerines.
Still, orange, red, and yellow objects might still look different
to them: the colors have different brightnesses. Red may be seen by
them as a faint green; yellow a stronger one. If they seem to be
able to discriminate red and yellow, they are noticing a difference
in the amount of light these colors
reflect toward them.
To imagine what this might be like, consider
the time of day when our color system breaks down: in the dusk
right before night. If you're outside in a park, in your yard, or
anywhere nature lives, take a look around you. You might notice the
wash of exuberant green leaves above you subtly dulling to a more
unassuming hue. You can still see the ground underfoot, but
details—the distinctness of blades of grass, the layers of
petals—are reduced. Depth of field squashes somewhat. I tend to
stumble more than usual on protuberant gray rocks that blend with
the earth. The reason for this loss of visual information is
anatomical. Cones, clustered toward the center of the retina, are
not sensitive to low light, so they don't fire as often at dusk or
night. As a result, our brains get signals from fewer cells
detecting colors. And the near world flattens out a little: we can
still see that there is color, and we still detect lights and
darks, but the richness of colors has fallen away; colors are
grainier, less detailed. So it might be for dogs, even at
noontime.
As they do not experience a great range of
distinct colors, dogs rarely show color preferences. Your clashing
choice of red leash and blue collar affects your dog not at all.
But a deeply saturated color may get more attention from a dog, as
will an object placed in a background of contrasting colors. It may
be meaningful that your dog attacks and pops all the blue or red
balloons left over after the birthday party winds down: they are
most distinct among a sea of pastels.
Go get the green bouncing ball … on the TV!
Dogs make up for their dearth of cones with a
battery of rods, the other kind of photoreceptor in the retina.
Rods fire most in low-light situations and at changes in light
densities, which is seen as motion. In human eyes, rods cluster at
the periphery, helping us notice something moving out of the corner
of our vision, or when the cones slow their firing at dusk or
night. The density of rods in dogs' eyes varies, but they have as
much as three times as many rods as we do. You can make that ball
your dog is not seeing directly in front of him magically appear by
giving it a little shove. Acuity greatly improves for close objects
when they are bouncing.
All these differences in the dog's perception,
experience, and behavior result from some small changes in the
distribution of cells on the back of the canine eyeball. And there
is another small change that results in a large
difference—potentially more far-reaching than a change in focal
area or color vision. In all mammalian eyes, rods and cones make
electrical activity out of light waves by means of a change in the
pigment in the cells. The change takes time—a very small amount of
time. But in that time, a cell processing light from the world
cannot receive more light to process. The rate at which the cells
do this leads to what is called the "flicker-fusion" rate: the
number of snapshots of the world that the eyes take in every
second.
For the most part, we experience the world as
smoothly unfolding, not as a series of sixty still images every
second, which is our flicker-fusion rate. Given the pace at which
events that matter to us happen, this is usually plenty fast. A
closing door can be grabbed before it slams; a handshake received
before it is withdrawn in annoyance. To create a simulacrum of
reality, films—literally "moving pictures"—must exceed our
flicker-fusion rate only slightly. If they do, we do not notice
that they are just a series of static pictures projected in
sequence. But we will notice if an
old-fashioned (pre-digital) film reel slows down in the projector.
While ordinarily the images are being shown to us faster than we
can process them, when it slows we see the film flickering, with
dark gaps between the frames.
Similarly, fluorescent lights are so annoying
because they operate too close to the human flicker-fusion rate.
The electrical devices used to regulate the current in the light
function right at sixty cycles per second, which those of us with
slightly faster flicker-fusion rates can thus see as a flicker (and
hear as a buzz). All indoor lights fluorescently flicker to
houseflies, with extremely different eyes than ours.
Dogs also have a higher flicker-fusion rate
than humans do: seventy or even eighty cycles per second. This
provides an indication why dogs have not taken up a particular
foible of persons: our constant gawking at the television screen.
Like film, the image on your (non-digital) TV is really a sequence
of still shots sent quickly enough to fool our eyes into seeing a
continuous stream. But it's not fast enough for dog vision. They
see the individual frames and the dark space between them too, as
though stroboscopically. This—and the lack of concurrent odors
wafting out of the television—might explain why most dogs cannot be
planted in front of the television to engage them. It doesn't look
real.*
One could say that dogs see the world faster
than we do, but what they really do is see just a bit more world in every second. We marvel at dogs'
seemingly magical skill at catching a Frisbee on the fly, or
following a rapidly bouncing ball. Their Frisbee-catching
procedure, as has been documented with microvideo recording and
trajectory analysis, turns out to match nicely the navigational
strategy naturally used by baseball outfielders to line themselves
up with the arc of an incoming ball. Excepting a few phenomenal
outfielders, dogs actually see the Frisbee's, or the ball's, new
location a fraction of a second before we do. Our eyes are
internally blinking in those milliseconds that a flung Frisbee is
moving along its course toward our heads.
Neuroscientists have identified an unusual
brain disorder in some humans called "akinetopsia." These
akinetopsics have a kind of motion blindness: they have difficulty
integrating a sequence of images into the normal perception of
motion. A person with akinetopsia may begin pouring a cup of tea
and then not register a change until many images later, by which
time the cup is overflowing. As non-brain-damaged persons are to
akinetopsics, dogs are to us: they see the interstices between our
moments. We must always seem a little slow. Our responses to the
world are a split second behind the dogs'.
VISUAL UMWELT
With age, Pump suddenly became reluctant to
enter the elevator, perhaps not seeing it well in the darkness
after being outside. I encourage her, or jump in myself first, or
throw something light-colored on the elevator floor for her to see.
Finally, every time, she rallies and leaps in, as though crossing a
great crevasse, brave girl.
So dogs can see some of the same things we do,
but they don't see in the way that we do. The very construction of
their visual capacity explains a broad swath of dog behavior.
First, with a wide visual field, they see what is around them well,
but what is right in front of them less well. Their own paws are
probably not in terrific focus to dogs. What wonder then how little
they use their paws, relative to our reliance on the end of our
forelimbs, to manipulate the world. A small change in vision leads
to less reaching, grabbing, and handling.
Similarly, dogs can bring our faces into
focus, but detect eyes less well. This means they will catch your
full facial expression better than a meaningful glare, and they
will follow a point or a turn better than a surreptitious glance
out of the corner of the eye. Their vision complements their other
senses. While they can locate a sound in space only roughly, their
hearing is good enough for them to turn their eyes in the right
direction, so they can search further visually … and then examine
closely by nose.
For instance, dogs recognize us by our
smells—but they also clearly look at us. What are they seeing? If
your smell is not available—you are downwind or you're covered in
perfume—they can use visual cues exclusively. They will hesitate if
they hear your voice calling them, but it is not your face atop the
approaching person, or your particular walk, or your mouth moving
to call their name. Recent research confirmed this by examining
dogs' behavior when they heard their owner's voice or a stranger's
voice, accompanied by either a picture of the owner's face (on a
large monitor) or of a stranger's face. The dogs looked longer at
the incongruous faces: the owner's face, when paired with a
stranger's voice, and the stranger's face, when it appeared with
the owner's voice. If it were just that the dog preferred the
owner's face, they would have always gazed at that face the
longest. Instead they looked longest when there was something
surprising: a mismatch.
The physical elements of vision define and
circumscribe what the dog experiences. There is a further element
of that experience: the role vision plays in the hierarchy of
senses. For visual creatures like humans, there is particular
delight when we encounter something through one of our non-visual
senses first. To arrive outside my apartment door and smell
something wonderful—to open the door and hear the sounds of
sizzling in pots, the clank of silverware; to be instructed to
taste a forkful of the pot's contents with my eyes closed—renders a
familiar experience new. I only come forth to verify the scene with
my eyes: my boyfriend in front of me with dinner in messy
preparation around him.
Coming to something through the secondary
senses first discombobulates, then introduces a feeling of novelty
to the ordinary. As dogs have their own hierarchy of senses, I
imagine that they too might feel the mystery of coming at something
by means other than their nose. This may explain both the
difficulty dogs have in understanding some of our first requests to
them (off the couch! I said to my new
puppy, as she looked at me searchingly), and the pride they seem to
take in learning a distinction from our visual world.
Though our visual worlds overlap, dogs attach
different meanings to the objects seen. A Seeing Eye dog must be
taught the umwelt of the human: the objects that are important to
the blind person, not those of interest to the dog. Try yourself
getting your dog to even acknowledge the existence of a sidewalk
curb. What is a curb to a dog? With persistence, dogs can be
taught, but most dogs simply do not see
a curb: it is not that the curb is invisible, but that it lacks any
important meaning to them. The surface below their feet may be
rough or soft, slippery or rocky, it may hold the scent of dogs or
of men; but the distinction between the sidewalk and the street is
a human distinction. A curb is but a slight variation in altitude
of the hardened mass with which we cover the dirt, which only has a
meaning to those who concern themselves with such concepts as
roads, pedestrians, and traffic. The guide dog must learn the importance of
the curb to his companion. He must learn the significance of a
speeding car, a mailbox, other people approaching, a doorknob. And
he will: he may begin to associate the curb with the distinctive
striping of a crosswalk, with the dark, smelly rain gutters that
run along them, or with the change in brightness from the concrete
to the asphalt. Dogs are much better at learning about things that
are important to us in our visual world than we seem to be in
understanding theirs. I still can't tell you why Pump became
excited at the mere sight of a husky-shaped dog appearing around
the corner. But after a dozen years I began to notice that she did.
She, on the other hand, was quicker to recognize the importance I
placed on certain objects—the distinction between the frayed sofa
and my favored armchair with respect to her chance of sitting on
it; the slippers whose fetching made me laugh versus the running
shoes whose delivery made me scold.
There is a final, unexpected facet of the
visual experience of the dog: they see details that we cannot. The
fact of dogs' relatively weak visual capacity turns out to be a
boon to them. Since they are not trying to take in the whole world
with their eyes alone, they may see details that we don't notice.
Humans are gestalt lookers: every time we enter a room, we take it
all in in broad strokes: if everything is more or less where we
expect it to be … yes … we stop looking. We don't examine the scene
for small, or even radical changes; we might miss a gaping hole in
the wall. Don't believe it? At every moment of our lives we are not
noticing a gaping hole: one in our visual field caused by the very
construction of our eyes. The optic nerve, the neural route
conveying information from the retinal cells to brain cells,
tunnels right through the retina on its way back to the brain. Thus
if we hold our eyes still, there is a part of the scene in front of
us that is not captured on our retina—as there is no retina there
to capture it. It's a blind spot.
We never notice this gaping hole in front of
us because our imaginations fill in that spot with what we expect
to be there. Our eyes dart back and forth constantly and
unconsciously—movement called saccades—to further complete the visual scene. We
never experience the missing spot. In the same way, we also have a
blind spot for those things that are slightly different—but close
enough—to what we expect to see. As well-adapted visual creatures,
our brains are equipped to find the sense in the visual information
sent it, despite holes and incomplete information.
We are maybe too well adapted. Some of what we
overlook, animals see. The celebrated autistic scientist Temple
Grandin has demonstrated the reality of this with cows, for
instance. Often cows being led along wending chutes into the
slaughterhouse balk, kick, and refuse to proceed. As far as we
know, this is not because they understand what will happen in the
slaughterhouse. Instead there were small visual details that
surprised or frightened them. The reflection of light in a puddle;
an isolated yellow raincoat; a sudden shadow; a flag flapping in
the breeze: seemingly insignificant details. We are certainly able
to see these visual elements—but we do not notice them as cows
do.
Dogs are closer to those cows than to us.
Humans quickly label and categorize a scene. Walking to work along
a Manhattan street, the typical commuter is perfectly oblivious to
the world he is passing. He notices neither beggars nor
celebrities; startles neither to ambulances or parades; simply
sidesteps a crowd gathered to gape at … well, whatever it is crowds
gape at: I rarely stop to see. On most mornings, the route is
reduced to its landmarks; nothing else needs attending to. There is
good reason to believe that this is not how dogs think. The walk to
the park becomes familiar over time, but they don't stop looking.
They are much more struck by what they actually see, the immediate
details, than what they expect to see.
Given how dogs see, how do they apply their visual ability? Cleverly: they look at us. Once a dog has opened up his eyes to us, a remarkable thing happens. He starts gazing at us. Dogs see us, but the differences in their vision also seem to allow them to see things about us that even we do not see. Soon it seems they are looking straight into our minds.
Seen by a Dog
I am startled and a little flustered to look
up from my work and see Pump watching me, her eyes trained on mine.
There is a powerful pull to a dog who looks you in the eyes. I am
on her radar: it feels that she is looking not just at me, but
to—and into—me.
Look a dog in the eyes and you get the
definite feeling that he is looking back. Dogs return our gaze.
Their look is more than just setting eyes on us; they are looking
at us in the same way that we look at them. The importance of the
dog's gaze, when it is directed at our faces, is that gaze implies
a frame of mind. It implies attention. A gazer is both paying
attention to you and, possibly, paying attention to your own
attention.
At its most basic level, attention is a process of bringing forward some
aspects of all the stimuli bombarding an individual in a moment.
Visual attention begins with looking;
auditory attention with hearing: both
are possible for all animals with eyes and ears. Just having the
sensory apparatuses isn't sufficient to do what we generally mean
by paying attention, though:
considering what it is one has turned to look or hear.
When invoked by psychologists, attention is
treated not just as turning the head toward a stimulus, but as
something else in addition: a state of mind that indicates
interest, intent. In attending to someone else's head turns, one
may be demonstrating an understanding of the psychological states
of other people—a distinctively human skill. We attend to others'
attention because it helps to predict what that someone other will
do next, or what he can see and what he might know. One of the
deficits that many people with autism have is an inability, or lack
of inclination, to look at other people's eyes. As a result, they
aren't instinctively able to understand when other people are
paying attention—or how to manipulate others' attention.
The simple ability to focus on some things
while ignoring others is crucial for any animal: objects one sees,
smells, or hears may be more or less relevant for survival. Attend
to those that are relevant; ignore the rest of the visual landscape
or the confusion of sounds. Even with survival no longer our most
pressing concern, humans are constantly trying to direct, divert,
or attract attention. Some attention mechanism is required to do
all the ordinary things of our days: to listen to someone talking
to us, to plan a walking route to work, even to remember what one
was thinking a moment ago.
Dogs, social animals like us, and also more or
less relieved of survival pressures, surely have some interesting
mechanisms with which to attend to the world. By virtue of their
different sensory abilities, though, they are able to attend to
things we never notice, such as how our odor changes through the
day. Likewise, we focus carefully on things that dogs do not even
detect, such as subtle differences in language use.
But what distinguishes dogs from other
mammals, even other domesticated mammals, is the way that their
attention overlaps with ours. Like us, they pay attention to
humans: to our location, subtle
movements, moods, and, most avidly, to our faces. A popular
conception of animals is that if they look at us at all, it is from
fear or appetite, monitoring us as possible predator or prey. Not
true: the dog looks very particularly at humans.
Just how particularly is the subject of a mad
rush of contemporary research into dog cognitive abilities. This
research uses as markers the landmarks in the development of human
infants into human adults, which is well documented, and which
result is obvious: by adulthood, we all understand what it means to
pay attention. What the dog research is revealing is that dogs have
some of the same abilities that we do.
THE EYES OF A CHILD
For dogs and humans both, it all begins with a
few innate behavioral tendencies. Having and understanding
attention is not automatic, but it develops naturally from these
instincts. Human infants, like most animals, have a basic orienting
reflex: move, as best or as much as you can, toward a source of
warmth, food, or safety. Newborns turn their faces toward warmth
and suck: the rooting reflex. At that age, infants can do little
more. Ducklings, more precocious, relentlessly chase after the
first adult creature they see.* In both ducklings and humanlings,
this reflex relies on an early perceptual ability: having at least
noticed the presence of others. It is
an ability that helps us, in our first few years, learn about the
important fact of others' attention.
For humans, there is a reliable course of development through infancy of certain behaviors associated with this growing understanding of other people. It is all about learning to attend to the right—human—things in the world, and beginning to understand that others are attending, too. And it begins as soon as they open their eyes. Newborn babies can see, although not much. They are incredibly nearsighted: peering, cooing faces brought just inches from their own may be clear, but that is about the extent of clarity of the world. One of the first things infants notice is any faces nearby. In fact our brains have specialized neurons that fire when we see a face. Infants can detect, and prefer to look at, a face or something facelike—even three points forming a V—rather than other visual scenes. From early in their lives, infants stare longer* at that which interests them, the mother's face being among the first items of interest. Soon infants also learn to distinguish a face looking toward them from one looking away. This is a simple skill, but not a trivial one: out of the visual cacophony of the world, they must start noticing that there are objects, that some of those objects are alive, that some of those alive objects are of particular interest, and that some of those interesting live objects attend to you when they face you.
Once that is established, and their own visual
acuity improves, infants focus on the details in that face. They
delight at peekaboo: a game playing simply with the importance of
eyes. As psychologists have shown by sticking out their tongues and
making faces at infants, very young infants can imitate simple
expressions. Of course, these expressions don't have the meaning
that they will later (we must assume that the infant is not
actually sticking his tongue out spitefully at the psychologist,
though one might wish it so). Infants are simply learning to use
their facial muscles. By three months, they've got it, and they
start reacting to others by making faces and smiling socially. They
move their heads to look at other faces nearby. By nine months old
they are tracking other people's gaze and seeing where it lands.
They might use that gaze to find some object that they have asked
about, or that has been hidden from them. Soon they extend the line
of gaze into a point with finger, fist, or arm to request an
object, and by their first birthday, to show or share.
These behaviors reflect the infant's
burgeoning understanding that other people have attention,
attention that can light on objects of interest: a bottle, a toy,
or them. Between twelve and eighteen months, they begin to engage
in bouts of joint attention with others: locking eyes, then looking
to another object, then back to eye contact. This marks a
breakthrough: to achieve full "jointness" the infant must on some
level understand that not only are they both looking together, they
are attending together. They are
understanding that there is some invisible but real connection
between other people and the objects that are in their line of
vision. Once they do this, all hell can break loose. Infants can
start manipulating others' attention simply by gazing someplace.
They check where other people are looking and pointing, and they
begin to notice if adults are looking at them while they are doing
activities they want to share (or conceal). They will give an
anticipatory glance at an adult before pointing or showing
themselves. They work very hard to get attention looking at them.
And they may begin avoiding attention: going out of a room at key
moments, or concealing objects from an adult's view. (This prepares
them well for being difficult adolescents.)
We all become characteristically human by this
same developmental route. Within a few years an infant goes from
aimlessly looking out of new eyes, to looking meaningfully, to
gazing at others, to following the gaze of others. They happily
hold eye contact. Before long they are using gaze to get
information, to manipulate the gaze of others—by distraction, gaze
avoidance, or pointing—and to get attention. At some point, they
come to a realization about the fact of the mind behind someone
else's gaze.
THE ATTENTION OF ANIMALS
She comes within an inch of me and starts panting at me, eyes wide and unblinking, to tell me that she needs something.
Step-by-step, cognition researchers have been
tracing this developmental course with a new subject: non-human
animals. How much of the infant's trajectory is followed by
animals? After they open their eyes, do they look with intention?
Do they notice others' eyes? Do they understand the importance of
attention?
This is one facet of the study of animal
cognition, which asks what an animal subject understands about the
"mental states" of others. Most of the experimental tests run with
animals are of the kind we feel sure we humans excel at: tests of
physical and social cognition. Captive animals from sea slugs to
pigeons to prairie dogs to chimpanzees have been set into mazes;
presented with counting, categorizing, and naming tasks; asked to
discriminate, learn, and remember series of numbers and pictures.
Tasks are devised to see if they recognize, imitate, or deceive
others—or even recognize themselves. And in some tests, the
question is even more characteristically human: of the kind of
social thinking going on when animals interact—with members of
their own species, and with those of other species. When a caged
chimpanzee looks at a human attendant, is he considering anything
about the attendant? Does he wonder how to get her to open the door
(does he wonder anything at all?), or is he simply waiting to see
what this colorful, animate object nearby does that might be
relevant or interesting? Does a cat consider that mouse as an
agent, as an animal with a life—or does he see the mouse as a
moving meal that must be stopped and dismantled?
As we've touched on already, the subjective
experience of animals is notoriously difficult to get at
scientifically. No animal can be asked to relate its experience in
voice or on paper,* so behavior must be our guide. Behavior has its
pitfalls, too, since we cannot be positive that any two
individuals' similar behavior indicates similar psychological
states. For instance, I smile when I am happy … but I may also
smile out of concern, uncertainty, or surprise. You smile back at
me: it too might be happiness—or ironic detachment. To say nothing
of the near impossibility of determining whether your "happiness"
feels like mine does.
Still, even without having constant
verification of others' mental states, behavior is a good enough
guide to allow us to predict an animal's future behavior well
enough to interact peacefully and productively. Thus we study what
animals do—in particular, what they do that is like what humans do.
Since using and following attention is so important in human social
interaction, animal cognition researchers look for behaviors that
indicate that an animal is using attention.
Dogs have recently trotted gamely into experimental labs, controlled outdoor facilities, and onto data sheets meant to gather information about their abilities at using attention. The dogs are put in controlled settings, usually with one or more experimenters present, and a hidden, desirable object: a toy or a food treat. By varying the cues that they use to inform the dogs about the location of the treat, the experimenters aim to determine which ones are meaningful to the dogs.
The question for researchers is just how far
along these stages of the child's development of attention dogs go.
Attention begins with gaze, and gaze requires visual capacity. We
have already established what dogs can see; we know that they look.
Do they understand attention?
Mutual
gaze
A gaze is more than it seems: by gazing at
someone, one very nearly acts upon him.
As my students discover in their field experiments, eye contact
comes close to feeling like actual tactile contact. There are
undiscussed and yet widely shared rules governing eye contact with
others—violation of which may be seen as an act of aggression or of
intimacy. We may stare down someone in an attempt to subdue them,
or, alternatively, use a long, steady gaze to indicate a more
lustful interest.
With a little variation, this could as easily
describe how many non-human animals use eye contact. Between apes,
eye contact is steeped with importance: it can be used as an
aggressive action, and will be avoided by a submissive member of a
troop. To stare at a dominant animal is to invite yourself to be
attacked. Not only do chimpanzees avoid staring, they avoid being
stared at. Subordinate chimps carry themselves despondently,
looking down at the ground or their own feet and only furtively
glancing around them. In wolves, too, a direct stare may be taken
as a threat. So the "aggressive" element of eye contact is the same
as with humans. The variation is this: all non-human animals with
any meaningful visual capacities will turn their eyes to something
of interest—but if the thing-of-interest is a member of their
species, the social pressure of gazing usually deflects the gaze of
interest.
Thus we can expect that dogs might act
somewhat differently than we do with regards to mutual gaze. As
dogs evolved from a species in which a stare is most often a
threat, we might do best to consider their avoidance of eye contact
less an inability than a result of their evolutionary history. But
wait! Dogs do look at our faces. They look at each other in the
center of the face: at eye level. Most dog owners will report that
their dogs gaze at them directly in the eyes.*
So something changed with dogs. While the
threat of aggression prevents mutual gaze among wolves,
chimpanzees, and monkeys, for dogs the information to be gained by
looking us in the eyes is worth enduring any residual, ancient fear
that a stare might cause an attack. That humans respond well to a
dog gazing at them is a happy circumstance—and our bond with them
is thereby strengthened.
To be sure, it may be less "eye contact" than
"face contact."* Because of the superficial anatomy of the dog
eye—the lack of distinct iris and whites of the eye—specific eye
direction can often only be confirmed from closer range than
scientists' video cameras have gotten. Generations of dog breeders
have tended to prefer the trait of dark eyes in their charges. Dogs
with light-colored irises are often thought to look volatile or
sneaky—ironically because we can see clearly when they avoid eye
contact. By breeding out the light irises we do not eliminate
shiftiness, just our awareness of the
fact that dogs shift their gaze. Darting eyes become less
conspicuous. We sleep better at night with a calm-featured dog at
the foot of our bed than with a nervous eye-darter. For all intents
and purposes, though, we can say that a dog and human "mutually
gaze" when we turn our faces toward each other.
The primal pull of gaze still affects dogs'
behavior. If you stare unblinkingly at your dog, he may look away.
Approached by a dog who appears overly aggressive or overly
interested, a dog can diffuse some of that excitement by glancing
to the side. Your chastisement or accusation of your dog
accompanied by a glare may also provoke a demure averral of the
dog's gaze. Given the easily recognized shifty look of the guilty
man confronted by his accuser, it is no wonder that we attribute
the same to the gaze-avoiding dog. The refusal to look us in the
eyes contributes to a look of guilt—especially when we are already
certain they have done something to inspire it. Whether they are
themselves feeling guilt or atavism is not obvious.
But the fact that dogs will look us in the
eyes allows us to treat them as a little more human. We apply to
them the implicit rules that accompany human conversations. It is
not uncommon to see a dog owner pause from scolding a "bad dog" to
physically turn the dog's head back toward the owner's face. We
want dogs to look at us when we are talking to them—just as we use
gaze in human conversation, in which listeners look at the face of
the speaker more than the reverse. (Notably, we do not stare at
each other non-stop in conversation, and might feel unsettled if
someone did.) There is more direct eye contact among humans
speaking intimately or honestly, and we tend to extend that
conversational dynamic to our dogs. We call their names before
speaking to them, treating them like willing, if taciturn,
interlocutors.
Gaze
following
It doesn't happen at once, but not long after
bringing a dog or puppy to your home for the first time you might
notice something: nothing in the house is safe. Dogs train humans
to become suddenly tidy: putting away shoes and socks almost as
soon as they are removed; taking out the trash well before it heaps
high; leaving nothing on the floor that could fit into the gaping
mouth of a teething, excited, unrestrained pup. A temporary peace
may ensue. After all, you can put things behind closed doors, into
shut cabinets, and onto high shelves. Dogs look, baffled, in the
area from which the (shoe, takeout container, hat) has mysteriously
gone missing. But soon you will notice that the dog has learned
something new: you are the source of
the mysterious relocations—and you have a tendency to tip your
hand.
How? You look. When we pick up the sock and
set it down, we're not just attached to it by a hand; the action is
accompanied by a gaze. We look where we are going. Later, we may
look again at that safe sock spot when discussing the dog's earlier
thievery. Again our gaze reveals the location of the sock: the gaze
is itself information. We have already met this ability to use the
direction of another's gaze, so-called gaze
following, which infants do before reaching one year old.
Dogs do it even sooner.
A gaze that intends to share information is
simply a point done without hands. Following a point is a slightly
simpler ability. Certainly dogs see a lot of pointing and gesturing
as they observe human members of their families. This may be the
source of their gaze-following ability, or it may simply bring out
an innate ability to glean any information possible from our
behavior. Researchers have tested the limits of their ability,
natural or learned, in various experiments that put dogs in a
context where they can get information from a person's pointing
gesture. For instance, a biscuit or other desired food might be
hidden under one of two inverted buckets while the dog subject is
out of the room. When all odor cues are masked, the dog has to make
a decision which bucket to choose. If he chooses correctly, he is
rewarded with the food; if not, he is rewarded with nothing. A
person who knows which bucket to choose is standing
nearby.
Chimpanzees have been given variations of this
task in captive research settings. Surprisingly, though they seem
to follow points, they don't always do well at following gaze
alone.
Dogs perform admirably. They follow points,
points that reach across the pointer's body, points from behind the
body, and are even better if the point includes a finger further
signifying the baited bucket.* They haven't simply learned the
importance of an outstretched arm. Pointing with elbows, knees, and
legs also serves as information. Given even a momentary point—a
glance of a point—the information is theirs. They can follow the
pointed cue given by a life-sized video projection of their owner.
Though they have no arms with which to point themselves, they
outperform the chimps who have been tested. Best, dogs can use
simply the person's head direction—her gaze—to get information. You
may be able to hide that sock from your sock-coveting chimpanzee,
but your dog will spot it.
Where dogs' use of attention really gets interesting is in less overt cases. Not just when we point and they look, but when they have to decide how to inform us that they need to go outside—or want a ball tossed to them. Or they need to tell us some very important news about where a tasty treat fell out of their reach while we were out of the room. Play with humans is a rich context for the possible appearance of some of these abilities; experimental paradigms also manipulate the information that can be gleaned from others' attention. All signs indicate that dogs seem to understand how to get attention, how to make requests of us using attention, and what kind of inattention allows them to get away with bad behavior.
Attention-getting
The first of these abilities, when seen in
children, is called "attention-getting." Informally, you may know
it as anything that your dog does to interfere with what you are
currently trying to do. More formally, these are behaviors that are
sufficient to change the focus of someone else's attention, by
stepping into his visual field, making a discernible noise, or
making contact. Suddenly jumping on you is a familiar dog
attention-getting behavior, if not one that is well loved by the
jumped-upon. Barking is another. Their attention-getting means
aren't restricted to the quotidian, though. Less recognized means
include bumping, pawing, or simply orienting oneself right up in
front of someone else: what I have called an in-your-face in my data of dog play behaviors.
Guide dogs use "sonorous mouth licking"—audible slurps—to get the
attention of their visually impaired charges when needed. The
excitement of play sometimes leads them to come up with novel
techniques, too. My favorite sessions to observe are those in which
an eager but frustrated dog mirrors the behavior of the object of
his unrequited play interest: approaching and drinking from the
bowl from which the first dog is drinking—and using it as a means
toward licking his face; or grabbing a stick of his own when
another dog finds a good stick to be sufficient company.
Dogs use attention-getters regularly with us,
and are often rewarded with our attention. But unless they show
some subtlety in application of these behaviors, their use does not
prove their full understanding of our attention. It may be that
they are simply throwing all the tools they have at the problem of
needing you to look at them. A child hollers, you come racing to
his side: an attention-getter is born. Observations of dogs playing
with humans show just how crude or subtle their use of these
behaviors is. There are dogs who will stand barking continuously
over a retrieved tennis ball, while their owners socialize with
members of their own species. While a good attention-getter, a bark
is not well applied if it continues to be used after it has failed
to get attention. On the other hand, there is also evidence of very
subtle visual attention-getters by dogs in reaction to the divided
attention of their owners. By changing posture, as from a seated
posture to standing, or from standing to approaching, dogs are able
to reengage the owner enough to toss a ball or lunge
playfully.
You are regularly a witness to the flexibility
dogs show in attention-getting. If your dog did not rouse you from
your armchair and novel by simply approaching you, he may have
wandered off only to return carrying a shoe or another verboten
item. Probably this causes you to chastise him gently and return to
your book. More serious tactics are needed, he sees. Next may come
whining, or a tentative woof; a tactile intervention—a slight push
with a wet nose, nuzzling, or jumping; even a loud drop to the
floor at your feet with a sigh. They are trying their best with you
here.
Showing
So far, the dogs have kept pace with the
developing child: gazing, following a point, following gaze, and
using attention-getters. Do they also point, as best they can, with
their bodies? Do they point with their heads to show you something?
Here again, experimenters set up a situation
that they presumed would prompt the behavior, if the ability
exists. The scenario is the gaze-following task, inverted. Instead
of being the naïve ones, in these cases the dogs are informed but
impotent: they alone witness an experimenter hiding a treat,
damnably out of their reach. Their owners then wander into the
room, and experimenters train their cameras on the dogs: Do they
see the owners as tools which can help them? If so, do they
communicate the location of the treat?
In these cases, it seems the only obtuse
animals in the room are the humans, who may not see the dog's
behavior as potentially showing them anything. That behavior
consists of a lot of attention-getters (such as barking), followed,
critically, by looking back and forth between the owner and the
location of the treat. In other words, pointing with that gaze:
showing.
This is visible daily in non-experimental
settings. Ball dogs crazy about retrieving generally deliver their
slobbered spheres to the front side—the face—of the ball tosser,
not to her back. And, if the ball is mistakenly dropped at the
unresponding backside of the owner, the dog has an arsenal of
attention-getters to employ, followed by relentless gaze
alterations—looking at the face of the human holding the ball, and
looking back at the ball in quick succession. The restless,
attention-starved dog is never satisfied dropping found socks at
your back; they are left within your sight, if not right on your
lap.
Manipulating attention
Finally, dogs use the attention of others as
information, both to get something they want and, more remarkably,
to determine when they can get away with something.
Research has determined this by asking if dogs
choose intelligently when given a choice about whom to request food
from. If every person is an equally good source of food, one would
expect that dogs would approach all persons with that same
beguiling expression—half entreaty, half expectation. There are
dogs who do so, of course,* and those who reserve their begging for
butchers, or owners who stuff their pockets with liver treats. But
most dogs make a distinction that is important to us when we desire
something: between possible and impossible collaborators. We make
requests appropriate to the state of knowledge and capacity of our
audience. You do not ask the baker to explain string theory and the
physicist for a loaf of seven-grain, sliced.
In experimental settings mining the same four
elements of dog, experimenter, food, and knowledge, dogs seem to
distinguish between humans who might be helpful to them and humans
who will likely not. When a person with a sandwich is either
blindfolded or facing away, dogs suppress the urge to stay as close
as possible to the sandwich. Instead, if there is a non-blindfolded
person nearby, they go beg to him instead. Let this be a lesson
that begging at the table is probably encouraged by your eye
contact toward the dog—even just long enough to tell him
no begging! Alternately, set up one
person as the responsive, looking beggee and all the dog's
attention will go to him. (Children are good for this
role.)
Dogs also approach the blindfolded persons
warily—as befits the situation, if one isn't let in on the fact of
being a subject in an experiment. These experiments using
unresponsive, oddly outfitted characters are typical of
psychological tests. At some level, they are useful in order to
avoid the possibility that the subject has had experience with the
setting they are about to encounter. In other words, the tests aim
to get at what the dogs intuitively understand about the knowledge
states of the human, not what the dog might have learned about what
to do when you see someone who is blindfolded. Still, the dog is
confronted with what must be a strange couple of hours.
Variations of the begging trials were first
run with chimpanzees. In that context, the attentional state of the
human was taken to indicate something about her knowledge. Someone
who sees food baited in one of two hidden bins is "knowledgeable";
someone who stands idly by in the same room, but has a bucket over
her head, is not. Did the chimps then beg to the knowledgeable
person or to the one who is guessing at the location of the food
(by chance guessing correctly once in a while)? Over time, chimps
learn to beg to the knowledgeable informant—but only when the
guesser has been out of the room, or has her back turned when the
bin is baited. When the guesser simply has her eyes blocked—with a
bucket, paper bag, or blindfold—the chimps begged to her,
too.
Dogs have gone through trials with odd humans
wearing buckets, blindfolds, or holding books in front of their
eyes, blocking their vision. They outperform chimps: dogs
preferentially beg to the looking—to those whose eyes they can see.
This is just how we act, preferring to talk, cajole, invite, or
solicit those whose eyes are visible. Eyes equal attention equals
knowledge.
Best, dogs use this knowledge for manipulative
ends. Researchers have found that dogs not only understand when we
are attentive, but are sensitive to what they can get away with at
different levels of their owners' attention. In one experiment,
after being instructed to lie down (and
dutifully so doing), dogs were observed in three trials. In the
first condition, an owner stood and stared at her dog. The result?
The dog stayed lying down: perfectly obedient. In the second
condition, the owner proceeded to sit down and watch television:
here the dog paused, but shortly disobeyed and got up. And in the
third condition, the owner didn't just ignore the dog but left the
room entirely, leaving the dog alone with his owner's command still
echoing in his ears.
Apparently the echo was not long-lasting, for
in these trials the dogs were quickest and likeliest to disobey the
same command so well heeded when the owner was around. What is
surprising is not that the dogs disobeyed when the owner left. It
is, instead, that dogs do what two-year-olds, chimps, monkeys, and
no other animals seem to do: simply notice exactly how attentive
someone is, and vary their own behavior accordingly. The dogs
methodically used the level of their owners' attention to determine
under what circumstances they were free to break the owners'
rules—just as they used the information from other dogs to get
attention back toward them in play.
The dogs' attention-reading is highly
contextual, however. When the same experiment was run using food,
that great motivator to perform at their best, the threshold to
disobedience was lowered: dogs disobeyed more quickly, and at lower
levels of owner distraction. When the owner's attention was harder
to gauge—when she was talking with someone else, or sitting quietly
with closed eyes—the dogs' behavior was mixed. Some sat patiently,
but, seemingly gathering steam, were prepared to spring up at once
as soon as the owner left the room. Other dogs took even longer to
disobey when the owners left the room than when they were in it but
otherwise engaged. This illogic might be explained by a
developmental fact, one that would vary dog to dog. Some owners
establish a routine of a sequence of commands: sit! stay! (long torturous pause), okay! In that routine, one might have to wait an
awfully long time before being given the okay to go at the food.
Dogs put up with this game of ours with admirable self-possession.
But if the owner starts chatting with someone else in the
room—busying himself with someone else's attention—why, the game is
off.
Lest you think that you can use this knowledge
to trick your dog into behaving himself while you are at work by
simply pretending to be home with him—over speakerphone or
video—one experiment brings very disappointing news. When a
life-sized video image (in visible digital) of the owner was
displayed before dogs, they disobeyed at levels befitting being
home alone with no supervision. While they could use their
video-owners' pointing hints to help find food, they didn't bother
to follow many of their verbal commands. Dogs are dutiful, but more
selectively dutiful when the owner is reduced to a videotape. You
cannot hope to reduce your dog's lonely wailing by telling him to
stop over the answering machine—but you might be able to tell him
where he can find that treat you left out for him. When you next
visit the zoo, check in on the monkey cages. Maybe there are
capuchin monkeys, quick-moving, tail-flaunting animals who leap
easily and shriek piercingly. Or colobus, slow-moving leaf-eaters
whose black-and-white coats often hide a small colobus clinging
inside. Watch the male snow monkeys as they follow around the
red-bottomed females. There is much to recognize here in our
distant evolutionary cousins. We see their interests, their fears,
their lusts. And most will notice and respond to you—most likely by
moving farther away, or turning their heads to avoid your gaze.
What is surprising is that dogs, so much less humanlike than these
primates, are so much better at realizing what is behind our gaze,
how to use it to get information or to their advantage. Dogs can
see us as our primate cousins cannot.
Canine Anthropologists
I am I because my little
dog knows me.
—GERTRUDE STEIN The dog's gaze is an
examination, a regard: a gaze at another animate creature. He sees
us, which might imply that he thinks about us—and we like to be
considered. Naturally we wonder, in that moment of shared gaze, Is
the dog thinking about us the way we are thinking about the dog?
What does he know about us?
We are known by our dogs—probably far better
than we know them. They are the consummate eavesdroppers and
peeping Toms: let into the privacy of our rooms, they quietly spy
on our every move. They know about our comings and goings. They
come to know our habits: how long we spend in the bathroom, how
long we spend in front of the television. They know whom we sleep
with; what we eat; what we eat too much of; whom we sleep too much
with. They watch us like no other animal watches us. We share our
homes with uncounted numbers of mice, millipedes, and mites: none
bothers to look our way. We open our door and see pigeons,
squirrels, and assorted flying bugs; they barely notice us. Dogs,
by contrast, watch us from across the room, from the window, and
out of the corner of their eyes. Their watching is enabled by a
subtle but powerful ability that begins with simple vision. Sight
is used to pay visual attention, and visual attention is used to
see what we attend to. In some ways
this is similar to us, but in other ways it surpasses human
capacity.
The blind and the deaf sometimes keep dogs to
see or hear the world for them. For some disabled persons, a dog
may enable movement through a world they cannot navigate alone.
Just as for the physically impaired dogs can act as eyes, ears, and
feet, so also do they act as readers of human behavior for some
autistic individuals. Persons with any kind of autism spectrum
disorder are united by their shared inability to understand the
expressions, emotions, and perspectives of other people. As the
neurologist Oliver Sacks describes, for an autistic person who
keeps dogs, the dogs may seem to be human-mind-readers. While an
autistic person cannot parse a brow furrowed with concern, or
interpret the rising tone indicating someone's fright or worry, the
dog is sensitive to the mind-set behind them.
Dogs are anthropologists among us. They are
students of behavior, observing us in the way that the science of
anthropology teaches its practitioners to look at humans. As
adults, we walk among other humans largely without examining them
closely, socially trained to keep to ourselves. Even with those we
know best, we might stop attending to the minute changes in their
expressions, their moods, their outlooks. The Swiss psychologist
Jean Piaget suggested that as children we are little scientists,
forming theories about the world and testing them by acting. If so,
we are scientists who hone our skills only to later neglect them.
We mature by learning how people behave, but eventually we pay less
attention to how others are behaving at every instant. We outgrow
the habit of looking. A curious child stares with fascination at
the stranger limping down the street: he will be taught this is not
polite. A child might be enraptured by a swirl of fallen leaves on
the pavement; by adulthood, he will overlook it. The child wonders
at our crying, monitors our smiles, looks where we look; with age
we are all still able to do all this, but we fall out of the
habit.
Dogs don't stop looking—at the gimpy walk, at
a rush of leaves tumbling down the sidewalk, at our faces. The
urban dog may be bereft of natural sights, but he is rich in the
odd: the drunken man swerving through the crowd, the shouting
sidewalk preacher, the lame and destitute. All get long stares from
the dogs who pass them. What makes dogs good anthropologists is
that they are so attuned to humans: they notice what is typical,
and what is different. And, just as crucially, they don't become
inured to us, as we do—nor do they grow up to be us.
DOGS' PSYCHIC POWERS DECONSTRUCTED
This attunement to us feels magical. Dogs are
able to anticipate us—and, it seems, to know something essential
about us and others. Is this clairvoyance? A sixth sense?
I am reminded of the story of a horse. At the
turn of the twentieth century, the actions of the horse Hans, whose
ironic sobriquet, "Clever Hans," has come to stand both for what he
could not do, and as a warning against overattributions to animals,
helped shape the course of animal cognition research for the next
hundred years.
Hans, his owner claimed, could count. Shown an
arithmetic problem written on a blackboard, Hans tapped out the sum
on the earth with his hoof. Though he had been encouraged and
reinforced, using straightforward conditioning, for tapping, this
was not a rote response to predetermined questions: he was
excellent at all sums, with novel problems, and even when the
questioner was someone other than his trainer.
Such was the tenor of the time that this
discovered, presumed latent cognitive ability of horses created a
small furor. Animal trainers and academics alike were stumped as to
how Hans was doing it. It almost looked as though there were no
other explanation than that he was actually doing
arithmetic.
Finally, the trick—an inadvertent trick,
unknown even to his owner—was discovered by a psychologist named
Oskar Pfungst. When the questioner himself was prevented from
knowing the answer to the problem, Hans's math was wildly off. Hans
was not counting, and he was not psychic; he was simply reading the
behavior of his questioners. They unconsciously tipped him off to
the answer through small bodily movements: leaning forward or away
from the horse when he'd tapped the correct sum; relaxing their
shoulders and muscles of their face; inclining ever so slightly
until the answer was reached.
Clever Hans stood, and still stands, as a
cautionary tale against assigning to animals abilities that could
be explained by simpler mechanisms. But thinking about the dog's
use of attention reminds me of Hans's skill. While Hans was not
clever in the way advertised, he was remarkably clever at reading
the inadvertent signals given by the persons quizzing him. Before
an audience of hundreds, only Hans noticed his trainer leaning
forward, his body tensing and relaxing, which Hans had figured out
meant that he was to stop tapping his hoof. He attended to the very
cues that had information: an attention far greater than the human
spectators brought to the event.
Hans's preternatural sensitivity may have
ensued, paradoxically, from other deficits. Since he presumably
didn't have any notion about numbers or arithmetic, he was not
distracted by those stimuli. By contrast, our attention to the
seemingly salient details would lead us to miss the one clear
indication of the answer.
An experimental psychologist I've met who does
research with pigeons demonstrates this phenomenon when teaching
his undergraduate class. He shows the students a series of slides
of bar graphs with blue bars of various lengths set against a white
background. The slides fall into one of two categories, he says:
those that have some unspecified "x-ness" feature, and those that
don't. He points out which slides are members of the x-ness
category. He then puts it to the students to use the example slides
to figure out what the conditions for x-ness are.
After many minutes of frustrated and failing
attempts by the students, he reveals that pigeons trained on a set
of members with x-ness can without fail tell of a new graph whether
it satisfies this elusive criterion or not. The students shift
uncomfortably in their seats. Still, not a person comes up with the
answer. Finally, their professor fills them in: those slides that
are mostly blue are members of the x-ness category; those that are
more white are not.
The students are outraged: they've been
outsmarted by pigeons. In running this test with my psychology
classes, I find they also decry the task. Though no student has
ever come up with the answer, they all later complain that the
answer is unfair. They were looking for some complicated
relationship between the bars—one consistent with the kinds of
relationships between features that bar graphs are meant to
represent. But there is none. "X-ness" is simply "more blue." Only
pigeons, blissfully unaware of bar graphs, saw them for their
colors and perceived the true categories.
What dogs do is a version of what Hans and the
pigeons did. Anecdotal tales of this kind of phenomenon abound. One
trainer of search-and-rescue dogs put his hands on his hips in
exasperation when the dog was following the wrong path. Another
rubbed his chin uneasily. In both cases, the dogs learned to use
the cues of their trainers as information that they were off the
trail. (The trainers had to be trained to tone down their cuing.)
As we look for the more complex explanation for an event, or for
others' behavior, we may overlook clues that dogs see naturally. It
is less extrasensory perception than the well-added sum of their
ordinary senses. Dogs use their sensory skills in combination with
their attention to us. Without their interest in our attention,
they would not perceive the subtle differences in our strides and
body postures and stress levels as important bits of information.
It allows them to predict us and to reveal us.
READING US
The dog observes us, thinks about us,
knows us. Do they then have some
special knowledge about us, born of their attention to us and to
our attention? They do.
In a nonverbal way, dogs know who we are, they
know what we do, and they know some things about us unknown to
ourselves. We're knowable by look, and even more so by our smells.
Over and above that, how we act defines who we are. Part of my
recognition of Pump is not just of her visage; it is of her walk:
her slightly off-kilter, jaunty trot with her droopy ears bouncing
in step. For dogs, too, the identity of a person is not just how
she smells and looks; it is how she moves. We are recognizable by
our behavior.
Even our most ordinary behavior—walking across
a room in our characteristic style—is chock full of information
that the dog can mine. All dog owners watch their pups' growing
sensitivity to the rituals that precede going for what in many
dog-peopled households is called a W-A-L-K.*
Dogs quickly learn to recognize shoe donning,
of course; we come to expect that grabbing a leash or a jacket will
clue them in; a regular walk time explains their prescience; but
what if all you did was look up from your work or rise from your
seat before your dog was on to you?
If done suddenly, or if you cross the room
with a purposeful stride, an attentive dog has all the information
he needs. Habitual watcher of your behavior, he sees your intent
even when you think you are giving nothing away. As we've seen,
dogs are very sensitive to gaze and thus to changes in our gaze.
The difference between a head lifted up or angled down, away from
them or toward them, is large for an animal so sensitive to eye
contact. Even small movements of the hands or adjustments of the
body attract notice. Spend three hours looking at a computer
screen, hands tethered to the keyboard, then look up and stretch
your arms overhead—this is a metamorphosis! The redirection of your
attention is clear—and a hopeful dog can easily interpret it as a
prelude to a walk. An acute human observer would notice this, too,
but we rarely let others oversee us so closely in our daily
affairs. (Nor do we find it terribly interesting to so
oversee.)
Their facility at anticipation of our actions
is part anatomy and part psychology. Their anatomy—all those rod
photoreceptors—allows them a millisecond head start on noticing
motion. They react before we see that there is something to react
to. The critical psychologies are of anticipation—predicting the
future from the past—and of association. Familiarity with your
typical movements is necessary in order to so anticipate you: a new
puppy might not be tricked by a feigned tennis-ball toss, but with
age he will be. Even without familiarity, dogs are skilled at
making associations between events—between the arrival of one's
mother and the delivery of food; between a shift in your focus and
the promise of a walk.
Dogs pick up the theme of our quotidian
habits, and thus are especially sensitive to variations in them.
Just as we often take the same route to our cars, to work, to the
subway, we take our dogs on similar walks. Over time, they learn
the route themselves, and can anticipate that we turn left past the
hedgerow and make a sharp right at the corner with the fireplug. If
we introduce a new detour on our way home, even an unnecessary
one—circling around the block an additional go—dogs adjust to the
new route after just a few outings. And they even begin heading in
the direction of the detour before their owner makes any movements
in that direction. This makes them fine, cooperative walking
companions—better than many humans I perambulate the city with,
whom I constantly knock into as I lead them on a preferred
route.
The complement of dogs' anticipatory prowess
is their purported character-reading ability. Plenty of people let
their dogs choose their potential romantic partners. Others declare
their dog a good judge of character, able to spot a duplicitous
person, a bad sort, on first meeting. They may seem to recognize
someone who is not to be trusted.* What this ability might come
down to is their close looking at our looking. If you feel hesitant
about an approaching stranger, you reveal it, however
unintentionally. Dogs are, as we have seen, sensitive to the
olfactory changes that come with stress; they can also notice
tensing muscles and the auditory change of quick breathing or
gasping. (These physiological changes are among those measured by
lie detectors: one might imagine that a trained dog could
substitute both for the machine and its technician.) But they will
let their visual ability trump these notes when assessing a new
person or trying to solve a problem. We all have characteristic
behaviors we display when angry, nervous, or excited.
"Untrustworthy" people often glance furtively in conversation. Dogs
notice this gaze. An aggressive stranger may make bold eye contact,
move unnaturally slowly or quickly, or veer oddly from a straight
path before doing any actual aggression. Dogs notice the behavior;
they react viscerally to the meeting of eyes.
One winter we took a trip north, to a place of
assertive winter and genuine cold, and were treated to a large
snowstorm. We pulled out sleds, found a great big hill, and
proceeded to plow an erratic track down it. Pump was suddenly
overcome, and ferociously pursued us on each ride downhill, biting,
grabbing, and growling at our faces. When it was my fast-moving,
snow-covered face being attacked, I couldn't stop her for all my
laughing. She was playing, but it was a play I have not seen
before: tinged with real aggression. When I managed to rise and
shake off the covering of snow I'd gathered on the way down, she
calmed at once.
Does this clairvoyance mean that dogs can't be fooled? No. They are astute watchers, but they are not mind readers, nor are they immune from being misled. I was changed for Pump when I leapt on that sled: I was horizontal; I was enrobed in snow gear and snow; and most critically, I was moving entirely differently. I was suddenly a smoothly moving, high-speed prey animal, not an upright, ambling companion.
My dog may have a particular interest in
sledders, but her behavior is similar to many other dogs' chasing
behaviors. Dogs often chase bicyclists, skateboarders,
RollerBladers, cars, or runners. The general-purpose answer given
for why they do this is usually that they have an instinct to chase
prey. This answer is not entirely wrong, but it is mightily
incomplete. It is not quite that dogs think of these objects or
persons as "prey," per se. Your motion reveals another dimension to
you: you roll!
quickly! It is an attribute that alters you in the dog's
eyes, which are especially responsive to a certain kind of motion.
Mounting and riding a bicycle, you have not turned to prey—as
indicated by the fact that your dog greets you, not eats you, on
dismount. Their responsive sensitivity probably evolved as a
prey-detection tactic, but it will be applied variously. It lends
to the dog's experience an additional way to interpret objects and
animals in the environment. That way is by the quality of their
motion.
There are shared components of sledding,
bicycling, or running: a person is moving in a certain way—smoothly
and quickly. Walkers are moving, but not quickly: they are not
chased. Pump did not recognize me sledding because ordinarily, much
as I would like to think otherwise, I am not particularly smooth
nor quick in my motions. There is an excess of vertical movement in
my walk; I weave to and fro; I gesture a lot—all frivolous in
making forward progress.
To stop a dog pursuing a bicycle with a
predatory glint in his eye, one can simply interrupt the illusion:
stop the bike. The chasing impulse triggered by the visual cells
that detected the motion will itself let up. (The hormones involved
in the arousal of barking and chasing such a smooth and quick mover
may still be coursing through his system, though, for a few
minutes.)
Science has confirmed the importance of
behavior in identity. Our identities, who we are, are defined
partly by our actions, so we can examine how actions inform
recognition of personal identity. In one experiment, dogs showed
that they have no difficulty distinguishing friendly and unfriendly
strangers: those demonstrating different identities. To do this,
the experimenters divided participants into two groups and asked
members of each group to behave in a prescribed manner. Friendly
behavior included walking at a normal speed, talking to the dog in
a cheerful voice, and gently petting the dog. Unfriendly behavior
included actions that could be interpreted as threatening: an
erratic, hesitating approach combined with staring at the eyes of
the dog without talking.
The main result of the experiment is not all
that surprising: the dogs approached the friendly and avoided the
unfriendly. But there's a hidden gem in the experiment. The key
trial was this: How did the dogs act when a formerly friendly
person suddenly acted threatening? The dogs acted variably: For
some, the person was now a different kind of person altogether—an
unfriendly one, her identity changed. To others, the olfactory
recognition of the stranger who had been friendly trumped this new
odd behavior.
These people began as strangers to the dog,
but over the course of the sessions, dogs became familiar with the
various people: they became "less strange." Their identity was
defined in part by their smell and in part by their
behavior.