INSIDE OF A DOG
WHAT DOGS SEE, SMELL, AND KNOW
ALEXANDRA HOROWITZ
SCRIBNER
A Division of Simon & Schuster, Inc.
1230 Avenue of the Americas
New York, NY 10020
www.SimonandSchuster.com
Copyright © 2009 by Alexandra Horowitz, with illustrations by the author
All rights reserved, including the right to
reproduce this book or portions thereof in
any form whatsoever. For information, address Scribner Subsidiary Rights
Department, 1230 Avenue of the Americas, New York, NY 10020.
First Scribner hardcover edition September 2009
SCRIBNER and design are registered trademarks
of The Gale Group, Inc., used under
license by Simon & Schuster, Inc., the publisher of this
work.
For information about special discounts for bulk purchases, please contact Simon & Schuster Special Sales: 1-866-506-1949 or business@simonandschuster.com.
The Simon & Schuster Speakers Bureau can
bring authors to your live event. For more information or to book
an event contact the Simon & Schuster Speakers
Bureau at 1-866-248-3049 or visit our website at www.simonspeakers.com.
Manufactured in the United States of
America
1 3 5 7 9 10 8 6 4 2
Library of Congress Control Number:
2008045842
ISBN 978-1-4165-8340-0
ISBN 978-1-4165-8827-6 (eBook)
To the dogs
Contents
Prelude
A prefatory note on the dog, training, and
owners
Calling a dog "the dog"
Training dogs
The dog and his owner
Umwelt: From the Dog's Point of Nose
Take my raincoat. Please.
A tick's view of the world
Putting our umwelt caps on
The meaning of things
Asking dogs
Dog kisses
Dogologist
Belonging to the House
How to make a dog: Step-by-step
instructions
How wolves became dogs
Unwolfy
And then our eyes met . . .
Fancy dogs
The one difference between breeds
Animals with an asterisk
Canis
unfamiliaris
Making your dog
Sniff
Sniffers
The nose nose
The vomeronasal nose
The brave smell of a stone
The smelly ape
You showed fear
The smell of disease
The smell of a dog
Leaves and grass
Brambish and brunky
Mute
Out loud
Dog-eared
The opposite of mute
Whimpers, growls, squeaks, and
chuckles
Woof
Body and tail
Inadvertent and intent
Dog-eyed
Eyes of the ball-holder
Go get the ball!
Go get the green ball!
Go get the green bouncing ball . . . on the
TV!
Visual umwelt
Seen by a Dog
The eyes of a child
The attention of animals
Mutual gaze
Gaze following
Attention-getting
Showing
Manipulating attention
Canine Anthropologists
Dogs' psychic powers deconstructed
Reading us
All about you
Noble Mind
Dog smarts
Learning from others
Puppy see, puppy do
More human than bird
Theory of mind
Theory of dog mind
Playing into mind
What happened to the Chihuahua
Non-human
Inside of a Dog
I. What a dog knows
Dog days (About time)
The inner dog (About themselves)
Dog years (About their past and
future)
Good dog (About right and wrong)
A dog's age (About emergencies and
death)
II. What it is like
It is close to the ground . . .
. . . It is lickable . . .
. . . It either fits in the mouth or it's too
big for the mouth . . .
. . . It is full of details . . .
. . . It is in the moment . . .
. . . It is fleeting and fast . . .
. . . It is written all over their faces . .
.
You Had Me at Hello
Bondables
Touching animals
At hello
The dance
The bond effect
The Importance of Mornings
Go for a "smell walk"
Train thoughtfully
Allow for his dogness
Consider the source
Give him something to do
Play with him
Look again
Spy on him
Don't bathe your dog every day
Read the dog's tells
Pet friendly
Get a mutt
Anthropomorphize with umwelt in mind
Postscript: Me and My Dog
Notes and Sources
Acknowledgments
Index
Outside of a dog, a book is man's best
friend.
Inside of a dog, it's too dark to read.
—ATTRIBUTED TO GROUCHO MARX
INSIDE OF A DOG
Prelude
First you see the head. Over the crest of the
hill appears a muzzle, drooling. It is as yet not visibly attached
to anything. A limb jangles into view, followed in unhasty
succession by a second, third, and fourth, bearing a hundred and
forty pounds of body between them. The wolfhound, three feet at his
shoulder and five feet to his tail, spies the long-haired
Chihuahua, half a dog high, hidden in the grasses between her
owner's feet. The Chihuahua is six pounds, each of them trembling.
With one languorous leap, his ears perked high, the wolfhound
arrives in front of the Chihuahua. The Chihuahua looks demurely
away; the wolfhound bends down to Chihuahua level and nips her
side. The Chihuahua looks back at the hound, who raises his rear
end up in the air, tail held high, in preparation to attack.
Instead of fleeing from this apparent danger, the Chihuahua matches
his pose and leaps onto the wolfhound's face, embracing his nose
with her tiny paws. They begin to play.
For five minutes these dogs tumble, grab,
bite, and lunge at each other. The wolfhound throws himself onto
his side and the little dog responds with attacks to his face,
belly, and paws. A swipe by the hound sends the Chihuahua scurrying
backward, and she timidly sidesteps out of his reach. The hound
barks, jumps up, and arrives back on his feet with a thud. At this,
the Chihuahua races toward one of those feet and bites it, hard.
They are in mid-embrace—the hound with his mouth surrounding the
body of the Chihuahua, the Chihuahua kicking back at the hound's
face—when an owner snaps a leash on the hound's collar and pulls
him upright and away. The Chihuahua rights herself, looks after
them, barks once, and trots back to her owner.
These dogs are so incommensurable with each
other that they may as well be different species. The ease of play
between them always puzzled me. The wolfhound bit, mouthed, and
charged at the Chihuahua; yet the little dog responded not with
fright but in kind. What explains their ability to play together?
Why doesn't the hound see the Chihuahua as prey? Why doesn't the
Chihuahua see the wolfhound as predator? The answer turns out to
have nothing to do with the Chihuahua's delusion of canine grandeur
or the hound's lack of predatory drive. Neither is it simply
hardwired instinct taking over.
There are two ways to learn how play works—and
what playing dogs are thinking, perceiving, and saying: be born as
a dog, or spend a lot of time carefully observing dogs. The former
was unavailable to me. Come along as I describe what I've learned
by watching.
I am a dog person.
My home has always had a dog in it. My
affinity for dogs began with our family dog, Aster, with his blue
eyes, lopped tail, and nighttime neighborhood ramblings that often
left me up late, wearing pajamas and worry, waiting for his
midnight return. I long mourned the death of Heidi, a springer
spaniel who ran with excitement—my childhood imagination had her
tongue trailing out of the side of her mouth and her long ears
blown back with the happy vigor of her run—right under a car's
tires on the state highway near our home. As a college student, I
gazed with admiration and affection at an adopted chow mix Beckett
as she stoically watched me leave for the day.
And now at my feet lies the warm, curly,
panting form of Pumpernickel—Pump—a
mutt who has lived with me for all of her sixteen years and through
all of my adulthood. I have begun every one of my days in five
states, five years of graduate school, and four jobs with her
tail-thumping greeting when she hears me stir in the morning. As
anyone who considers himself a dog person will recognize, I cannot
imagine my life without this dog.
I am a dog person, a lover of dogs. I am also
a scientist.
I study animal behavior. Professionally, I am
wary of anthropomorphizing animals, attributing to them the
feelings, thoughts, and desires that we use to describe ourselves.
In learning how to study the behavior of animals, I was taught and
adhered to the scientist's code for describing actions: be
objective; do not explain a behavior by appeal to a mental process
when explanation by simpler processes will do; a phenomenon that is
not publicly observable and confirmable is not the stuff of
science. These days, as a professor of animal behavior, comparative
cognition, and psychology, I teach from masterful texts that deal
in quantifiable fact. They describe everything from hormonal and
genetic explanations for the social behavior of animals, to
conditioned responses, fixed action patterns, and optimal foraging
rates, in the same steady, objective tone.
And yet.
Most of the questions my students have about
animals remain quietly unanswered in these texts. At conferences
where I have presented my research, other academics inevitably
direct the postlecture conversations to their own experiences with
their pets. And I still have the same questions I'd always had
about my own dog—and no sudden rush of answers. Science, as
practiced and reified in texts, rarely addresses our experiences of
living with and attempting to understand the minds of our
animals.
In my first years of graduate school, when I
began studying the science of the mind, with a special interest in
the minds of non-human animals, it never occurred to me to study
dogs. Dogs seemed so familiar, so understood. There is nothing to
be learned from dogs, colleagues claimed: dogs are simple, happy
creatures whom we need to train and feed and love, and that is all
there is to them. There is no data in
dogs. That was the conventional wisdom among scientists. My
dissertation advisor studied, respectably, baboons: primates are
the animals of choice in the field of animal cognition. The
assumption is that the likeliest place to find skills and cognition
approaching our own is in our primate brethren. That was, and
remains, the prevailing view of behavioral scientists. Worse still,
dog owners seemed to have already covered the territory of
theorizing about the dog mind, and their theories were generated
from anecdotes and misapplied anthropomorphisms. The very notion of
the mind of a dog was tainted.
And yet.
I spent many recreational hours during my
years of graduate school in California in the local dog parks and
beaches with Pumpernickel. At the time I was in training as an
ethologist, a scientist of animal behavior. I joined two research
groups observing highly social creatures: the white rhinoceros at
the Wild Animal Park in Escondido, and the bonobos (pygmy
chimpanzees) at the Park and the San Diego Zoo. I learned the
science of careful observations, data gathering, and statistical
analysis. Over time, this way of looking began seeping into those
recreational hours at the dog parks. Suddenly the dogs, with their
fluent travel between their own social world and that of people,
became entirely unfamiliar: I stopped seeing their behavior as
simple and understood.
Where I once saw and smiled at play between
Pumpernickel and the local bull terrier, I now saw a complex dance
requiring mutual cooperation, split-second communications, and
assessment of each other's abilities and desires. The slightest
turn of a head or the point of a nose now seemed directed,
meaningful. I saw dogs whose owners did not understand a single
thing their dogs were doing; I saw dogs too clever for their
playmates; I saw people misreading canine requests as confusion and
delight as aggression. I began bringing a video camera with us and
taping our outings at the parks. At home I watched the tapes of
dogs playing with dogs, of people ball-and Frisbee-tossing to their
dogs—tapes of chasing, fighting, petting, running, barking. With
new sensitivity to the possible richness of social interactions in
an entirely non-linguistic world, all of these once ordinary
activities now seemed to me to be an untapped font of information.
When I began watching the videos in extremely slow-motion playback,
I saw behaviors I had never seen in years of living with dogs.
Examined closely, simple play frolicking between two dogs became a
dizzying series of synchronous behaviors, active role swapping,
variations on communicative displays, flexible adaptation to
others' attention, and rapid movement between highly diverse play
acts.
What I was seeing were snapshots of the minds
of the dogs, visible in the ways they communicated with each other
and tried to communicate with the people around them—and, too, in
the way they interpreted other dogs' and people's
actions.
I never saw Pumpernickel—or any dog—the same
way again. Far from being a killjoy on the delights of interacting
with her, though, the spectacles of science gave me a rich new way
to look at what she was doing: a new way to understand life as a
dog.
Since those first hours of viewing, I have
studied dogs at play: playing with other dogs and playing with
people. At the time I was unwittingly part of a sea change taking
place in science's attitude toward studying dogs. The
transformation is not yet complete, but the landscape of dog
research is already remarkably different than it was twenty years
ago. Where once there was an inappreciable number of studies of dog
cognition and behavior, there are now conferences on the dog,
research groups devoted to studying the dog, experimental and
ethological studies on the dog in the United States and abroad, and
dog research results sprinkled through scientific journals. The
scientists doing this work have seen what I have seen: the dog is a
perfect entry into the study of non-human animals. Dogs have lived
with human beings for thousands, maybe hundreds of thousands of
years. Through the artificial selection of domestication, they have
evolved to be sensitive to just those things that importantly make
up our cognition, including, critically, attention to
others.
In this book I introduce you to the science of
the dog. Scientists working in laboratories and in the field,
studying working dogs and companion dogs, have gathered an
impressive amount of information on the biology of dogs—their
sensory abilities, their behavior—and on the psychology of
dogs—their cognition. Drawing from the accumulated results of
hundreds of research programs, we can begin to create a picture of
the dog from the inside—of the skill of his nose, what he hears,
how his eyes turn to us, and the brain behind it all. The dog
cognition work reviewed includes my own but extends far beyond it
to summarize all the results from recent research. For some topics
on which there is no reliable information yet on dogs, I
incorporate studies on other animals that might help us understand
a dog's life, too. (For those whose appetite for the original
research articles is whetted by the accounts herein, full citations
appear at the book's end.)
We do no disservice to dogs by stepping away
from the leash and considering them scientifically. Their abilities
and point of view merit special attention. And the result is
magnificent: far from being distanced by science, we are brought
closer to and can marvel at the true nature of the dog. Used
rigorously but creatively, the process and results of science can
shed new light on discussions that people have daily about what
their dog knows, understands, or believes. Through my personal
journey, learning to look systemically and scientifically at my own
dog's behavior, I came to have a better understanding of,
appreciation of, and relationship with her.
I've gotten inside of the dog, and have
glimpsed the dog's point of view. You can do the same. If you have
a dog in the room with you, what you see in that great, furry pile
of dogness is about to change.
A PREFATORY NOTE ON THE DOG, TRAINING, AND
OWNERS
Calling a dog "the dog"
It is the nature of scientific study of
non-human animals that a few individual animals who have been
thoroughly poked, observed, trained, or dissected come to represent
their entire species. Yet with humans we never let one person's
behavior stand for all of our behavior. If one man fails to solve a
Rubik's cube in an hour, we do not extrapolate from that that all
men will so fail (unless that man had bested every other man
alive). Here our sense of individuality is stronger than our sense
of shared biology. When it comes to describing our potential
physical and cognitive capacities, we are individuals first, and
members of the human race second.
By contrast, with animals the order is
reversed. Science considers animals as representatives of their
species first, and as individuals second. We are accustomed to
seeing a single animal or two kept in a zoo as representative of
their species; to zoo management, they are even unwitting
"ambassadors" of the species. Our view of the uniformity of species
members is well exemplified in our comparison of their
intelligence. To test the hypothesis, long popular, that having a
bigger brain indicates greater intelligence, the brain volumes of
chimpanzees, monkeys, and rats were compared with human brains.
Sure enough, the chimp's brain is smaller than ours, the monkey's
smaller than the chimp's, the rat's a mere cerebellum-sized node of
the primates' brains. That much of the story is fairly well known.
What is more surprising is that the brains used, for comparative
purposes, were the brains of just two or three chimpanzees and
monkeys. These couple of animals unlucky enough to lose their heads
for science were henceforth considered perfectly representative
monkeys and chimps. But we had no idea if they happened to be
particularly big-brained monkeys, or abnormally small-brained
chimps.*
Similarly, if a single animal or small group
of animals fails in a psychological experiment, the species is
tainted with the brush of failure. Although grouping animals by
biological similarity is clearly useful shorthand, there is a
strange result: we tend to speak of the species as though all
members of the species were identical. We never make this slip with
humans. If a dog, given the choice between a pile of twenty
biscuits and a pile often biscuits, chooses the latter, the
conclusion is often stated with the definite article: "the dog"
cannot distinguish between large and small piles—not "a dog" cannot
so distinguish.
So when I talk about the
dog, I am talking implicitly about those dogs studied to date. The results of many well-performed
experiments may eventually allow us to reasonably generalize to
all dogs, period. But even then, the
variations among individual dogs will be great: your dog may be an
unusually good smeller, may never look you in the eye, may love his
dog bed and hate to be touched. Not every behavior a dog does
should be interpreted as telling, taken as something intrinsic or
fantastic; sometimes they just are,
just as we are. That said, what I offer herein is the known
capacity of the dog; your results may
be different.
Training dogs
This is not a dog training book. Still, its
contents might lead you to be able to train your dog,
inadvertently. This will catch us up to dogs, who have already,
without a tome on people, learned how to train us without our
realizing it.
The dog training literature and the dog
cognition and behavior literatures do not overlap greatly. Dog
trainers do use a few basic tenets from psychology and
ethology—sometimes to great effect, sometimes to disastrous end.
Most training operates on the principle of associative learning. Associations between events
are easily learned by all animals, including humans. Associative
learning is what is behind "operant" conditioning paradigms, which
provide a reward (a treat, attention, a toy, a pat) after the
occurrence of a desired behavior (a dog sitting down). Through
repeated application, one can shape a
new, desired behavior in a dog—be it lying down and rolling over,
or, for the ambitious, calmly Jet-Skiing behind a
motorboat.
But often the tenets of training clash with
the scientific study of the dog. For instance, many trainers use
the analogy of dog-as-tame-wolf as informative in how we should see
and treat dogs. An analogy can only be as good as its source. In
this case, as we will see, scientists know a limited amount about
natural wolf behavior—and what we know often contradicts the
conventional wisdom used to bolster those analogies.
In addition, training methods are not
scientifically tested, despite some trainers' assertions to the
contrary. That is, no training program has been evaluated by
comparing the performance of an experimental group that gets
training and a control group whose life is identical except for the
absence of the training program. People who come to trainers often
share two unusual features: their dogs are less "obedient" than the
average dog, and the owners are more motivated to change them than
the average owner. It is very likely, given this combination of
conditions and a few months, that the dog will behave differently
after training, almost regardless of what the training
is.
Training successes are exciting, but they do
not prove that the training method is what led to the success. The
success could be indicative of good
training. But it could also be a happy accident. It could also be
the result of more attention being paid to the dog over the course
of the program. It could be the result of the dog's maturing over
the course of the program. It could be the result of that bullying
dog down the street moving away. In other words, the success could
be the result of dozens of other co-occurring changes in the dog's
life. We cannot distinguish these possibilities without rigorous
scientific testing.
Most critically, training is usually tailored
to the owner—to change the dog to fit the owner's conception of the
role of the dog, and of what he wants the dog to do. This goal is
quite different than our aim: looking to see what the dog actually
does, and what he wants from and understands of you.
The dog and his owner
It is increasingly in vogue to speak not of pet ownership but pet guardianship, or pet companions. Clever writers talk of dogs' "humans," turning the ownership arrow back on ourselves. In this book I call dogs' families owners simply because this term describes the legal relationship we have with dogs: peculiarly, they are still considered property (and property of little compensatory value, besides breeding value, a lesson I hope no reader ever has to learn personally). I will celebrate the day when dogs are not property which we own. Until then, I use the word owner apolitically, for convenience and with no other motive. This motive guides me in my pronoun use, too: unless discussing a female dog, I usually call the dog "him," as this is our gender-neutral term. The reputedly more neutral "it" is not an option, for anyone who has known a dog.
Umwelt: From the Dog's Point of Nose
This morning I was awakened by Pump coming
over to the bed and sniffing emphatically at me, millimeters away,
her whiskers grazing my lips, to see if I was awake or alive or me.
She punctuates her rousing with an exclamatory sneeze directly in
my face. I open my eyes and she is gazing at me, smiling, panting a
hello.
Go look at a dog. Go on, look—maybe at one
lying near you right now, curled around his folded legs on a dog
bed, or sprawled on his side on the tile floor, paws flitting
through the pasture of a dream. Take a good look—and now forget
everything you know about this or any dog.
This is admittedly a ridiculous exhortation: I
don't really expect that you could easily forget even the name or
favored food or unique profile of your dog, let alone everything
about him. I think of the exercise as analogous to asking a
newcomer to meditation to enter into satori, the highest state, on
the first go: aim for it, and see how far you get. Science, aiming
for objectivity, requires that one becomes aware of prior
prejudices and personal perspective. What we'll find, in looking at
dogs through a scientific lens, is that some of what we think we
know about dogs is entirely borne out; other things that appear
patently true are, on closer examination, more doubtful than we
thought. And by looking at our dogs from another perspective—from
the perspective of the dog—we can see new things that don't
naturally occur to those of us encumbered with human brains. So the
best way to begin understanding dogs is by forgetting what we think
we know.
The first things to forget are
anthropomorphisms. We see, talk about, and imagine dogs' behavior
from a human-biased perspective, imposing our own emotions and
thoughts on these furred creatures. Of
course, we'll say, dogs love and desire; of course they
dream and think; they also know and understand us, feel bored, get
jealous, and get depressed. What could be a more natural
explanation of a dog staring dolefully at you as you leave the
house for the day than that he is depressed that you're
going?
The answer is: an explanation based in what
dogs actually have the capacity to feel, know, and understand. We
use these words, these anthropomorphisms, to help us make sense of
dogs' behavior. Naturally, we are intrinsically prejudiced toward
human experiences, which leads us to understand animals'
experiences only to the extent that they match our own. We remember
stories that confirm our descriptions of animals and conveniently
forget those that do not. And we do not hesitate to assert "facts"
about apes or dogs or elephants or any animal without proper
evidence. For many of us, our interaction with non-pet animals
begins and ends with our staring at them at zoos or watching shows
on cable TV. The amount of useful information we can get from this
kind of eavesdropping is limited: such a passive encounter reveals
even less than we get from glancing in a neighbor's window as we
walk by.* At least the neighbor is of our own species.
Anthropomorphisms are not inherently odious.
They are born of attempts to understand the world, not to subvert
it. Our human ancestors would have regularly anthropomorphized in
an attempt to explain and predict the behavior of other animals,
including those they might want to eat or that might want to eat
them. Imagine encountering a strange, bright-eyed jaguar at dusk in
the forest, and looking squarely in its eyes looking squarely into
yours. At that moment, a little meditation on what you might be
thinking "if you were the jaguar" would probably be due—and would
lead to your hightailing it away from the cat. Humans endured: the
attribution was, if not true, at least true enough.
Typically, though, we are no longer in the
position of needing to imagine the jaguar's desires in time to
escape his clutches. Instead we are bringing animals inside and
asking them to become members of our families. For that purpose,
anthropomorphisms fail to help us incorporate those animals into
our homes, and have the smoothest, fullest relationships with them.
This is not to say that we're always wrong with our attributions:
it might be true that our dog is sad, jealous, inquisitive,
depressed—or desiring a peanut butter sandwich for lunch. But we
are almost certainly not justified in claiming, say, depression from the evidence before us: the
mournful eyes, the loud sigh. Our projections onto animals are
often impoverished—or entirely off the mark. We might judge an
animal to be happy when we see an upturn of the corners of his
mouth; such a "smile," however, can be misleading. On dolphins, the
smile is a fixed physiological feature, immutable like the creepily
painted face of a clown. Among chimpanzees, a grin is a sign of
fear or submission, the furthest thing from happiness. Similarly, a
human might raise her eyebrows in surprise, but the eyebrow-raising
capuchin monkey is not surprised. He is evincing neither skepticism
nor alarm; instead he is signaling to nearby monkeys that he has
friendly designs. By contrast, among baboons a raised brow can be a
deliberate threat (lesson: be careful which monkey you raise your
eyebrows toward). The onus is on us to find a way to confirm or
refute these claims we make of animals.
It may seem a benign slip from sad eyes to
depression, but anthropomorphisms often slide from benign to
harmful. Some risk the welfare of the animals under consideration.
If we're to put a dog on antidepressants based on our
interpretation of his eyes, we had better be pretty sure of our
interpretation. When we assume we know what is best for an animal,
extrapolating from what is best for us or any person, we may
inadvertently be acting at cross-purposes with our aims. For
instance, in the last few years there has been considerable to-do
made about improved welfare for animals raised for food, such as
broiler chickens who have access to the outside, or have room to
roam in their pens. Though the end result is the same for the
chicken—it winds up as someone's dinner—there is a budding interest
in the welfare of the animals before they are killed.
But do they want to range freely? Conventional
wisdom holds that no one, human or not, likes to be pressed up against others. Anecdotes
seem to confirm this: given the choice of a subway car jammed with
hot, stressed commuters, and one with only a handful of people, we
choose the latter in a second (heeding the possibility, of course,
that there's some other explanation—a particularly smelly person,
or a glitch in air-conditioning—that explains this favorable
distribution). But the natural behavior of chickens may indicate
otherwise: chickens flock. They don't sally forth on their
own.
Biologists devised a simple experiment to test
the chickens' preferences of where to be: they picked up individual
animals, relocated them randomly within their houses, and monitored
what the chickens did next. What they found was that most chickens
moved closer to other chickens, not farther away, even when there
was open space available. Given the option of space to spread their
wings … they choose the jammed subway car.
This is not to say that chickens thus
like being smushed against other birds
in a cage, or find it a perfectly agreeable life. It is inhumane to
pen chickens so tightly they cannot move. But it is to say that
assuming resemblance between chicken preferences and our
preferences is not the way to insight about what the chicken
actually does like. Not coincidentally, these broiler chickens are
killed before they reach six weeks of age; domestic chicks are
still being brooded by their mothers at that age. Deprived of the
ability to run under her wings, the broiler chickens run closer to
other chickens.
TAKE MY RAINCOAT. PLEASE.
Do our anthropomorphic tendencies ever miss so
fabulously with dogs? Without a doubt they do. Take raincoats.
There are some interesting assumptions involved in the creation and
purchase of tiny, stylish, four-armed rain slickers for dogs. Let's
put aside the question of whether dogs prefer a bright yellow
slicker, a tartan pattern, or a raining-cats-and-dogs motif
(clearly they prefer the cats and
dogs). Many dog owners who dress their dogs in coats have the best
intentions: they have noticed, perhaps, that their dog resists
going outside when it rains. It seems reasonable to extrapolate
from that observation to the conclusion that he dislikes the rain.
He dislikes the
rain. What is meant by that? It is that he must dislike
getting the rain
on his body, the way many of us do. But is that a sound
leap? In this case, there is plenty of seeming evidence from the
dog himself. Is he excited and wagging when you get the raincoat
out? That seems to support the leap … or, instead, the conclusion
that he realizes that the appearance of the coat predicts a
long-awaited walk. Does he flee from the coat? Curl his tail under
his body and duck his head? Undermines the leap—though does not
discredit it outright. Does he look bedraggled when wet? Does he
shake the water off excitedly? Neither confirmatory nor
disconfirming. The dog is being a little opaque.
Here the natural behavior of related, wild
canines proves the most informative about what the dog might think
about a raincoat. Both dogs and wolves have, clearly, their own
coats permanently affixed. One coat is enough: when it rains,
wolves may seek shelter, but they do not cover themselves with
natural materials. That does not argue for the need for or interest
in raincoats. And besides being a jacket, the raincoat is also one
distinctive thing: a close, even pressing, covering of the back,
chest, and sometimes the head. There are occasions when wolves get pressed upon the back
or head: it is when they are being dominated by another wolf, or
scolded by an older wolf or relative. Dominants often pin
subordinates down by the snout. This is called muzzle biting, and
accounts, perhaps, for why muzzled dogs sometimes seem
preternaturally subdued. And a dog who "stands over" another dog is
being dominant. The subordinate dog in that arrangement would feel
the pressure of the dominant animal on his body. The raincoat might
well reproduce that feeling. So the principal experience of wearing
a coat is not the experience of feeling protected from wetness;
rather, the coat produces the discomfiting feeling that someone
higher ranking than you is nearby.
This interpretation is borne out by most dogs'
behavior when getting put into a raincoat: they may freeze in place
as they are "dominated." You might see the same behavior when a dog
resisting a bath suddenly stops struggling when he gets fully
sodden or covered with a heavy, wet towel. The be-jacketed dog may
cooperate in going out, but not because he has shown he likes the
coat; it is because he has been subdued.* And he will wind up being
less wet, but it is we who care about the planning for that, not
the dog. The way around this kind of misstep is to replace our
anthropomorphizing instinct with a behavior-reading instinct. In
most cases, this is simple: we must ask the dog what he wants. You
need only know how to translate his answer.
A TICK'S VIEW OF THE WORLD
Here is our first tool to getting that answer:
imagining the point of view of the dog. The scientific study of
animals was changed by a German biologist of the early twentieth
century named Jakob von Uexküll. What he proposed was
revolutionary: anyone who wants to understand the life of an animal
must begin by considering what he called their umwelt (OOM-velt): their subjective or
"self-world." Umwelt captures what life is like as the animal. Consider, for instance, the lowly
deer tick. Those of you who have spent long minutes hesitatingly
petting the body of a dog for the telltale pinhead that indicates a
tick swollen with blood may have already considered the tick. And
you probably consider the tick as a pest, period. Barely even an
animal. Von Uexküll considered, instead, what it might be like from
the tick's point of view.
A little background: ticks are parasites.
Members of the family arachnid, a class that includes spiders and
insects, they have four pairs of legs, a simple body type, and
powerful jaws. Thousands of generations of evolution have pared
their life to the straightforward: birth, mating, eating, and
dying. Born legless and without sex organs, they soon grow these
parts, mate, and climb to a high perch—say, a blade of grass.
Here's where their tale gets striking. Of all the sights, sounds,
and odors of the world, the adult tick is waiting for just one. It
is not looking around: ticks are blind. No sound bothers the tick:
sounds are irrelevant to its goal. It only awaits the approach of a
single smell: a whiff of butyric acid, a fatty acid emitted by
warm-blooded creatures (we sometimes smell it in sweat). It might
wait here for a day, a month, or a dozen years. But as soon as it
smells the odor it is fixed on, it drops from its perch. Then a
second sensory ability kicks in. Its skin is photosensitive, and
can detect warmth. The tick directs itself toward warmth. If it's
lucky, the warm, sweaty smell is an animal, and the tick grasps on
and drinks a meal of blood. After feeding once, it drops, lays
eggs, and dies.
The point of this tale of the tick is that the
tick's self-world is different than ours in unimagined ways: what
it senses or wants; what its goals are. To the tick, the complexity
of persons is reduced to two stimuli: smell and warmth—and it is
very intent on those two things. If we want to understand the life
of any animal, we need to know what things are meaningful to it.
The first way to discover this is to determine what the animal can
perceive: what it can see, hear, smell, or otherwise sense. Only
objects that are perceived can have meaning to the animal; the rest
are not even noticed, or all look the same. The wind that whisks
through the grasses? Irrelevant to the tick. The sounds of a
childhood birthday party? Doesn't appear on its radar. The
delicious cake crumbs on the ground? Leave the tick cold.
Second, how does the animal act on the world?
The tick mates, waits, drops, and feeds. So the objects of the
universe, for the tick, are divided into ticks and non-ticks;
things one can or cannot wait upon; surfaces one might or might not
drop onto; and substances one may or may not want to feed
on.
Thus, these two components—perception and
action—largely define and circumscribe the world for every living
thing. All animals have their own umwelten—their own subjective realities, what von
Uexküll thought of as "soap bubbles" with them forever caught in
the middle. We humans are enclosed in our own soap bubbles, too. In
each of our self-worlds, for instance, we are very attentive to
where other people are and what they are doing or saying. (By
contrast, imagine the tick's indifference to even our most moving
monologues.) We see in the visual range of light, we hear audible
noises, and we smell strong odors placed in front of our noses. On
top of that each individual creates his own personal umwelt, full
of objects with special meaning to him. You can most clearly see
this last fact by letting yourself be led through an unknown city
by a native. He will steer you along a path obvious to him, but
invisible to you. But the two of you share some things: neither of
you is likely to stop and listen to the ultrasonic cry of a nearby
bat; neither of you smells what the man passing you had for dinner
last night (unless it involved a lot of garlic). We, the ticks, and
every other animal dovetail into our environment: we are bombarded
with stimuli, but only a very few are meaningful to us.
The same object, then, will be seen (or,
better, sensed—some animals do not see
well or at all) by different animals differently. A rose is a rose
is a rose. Or is it? To a human a rose is a certain kind of flower,
a gift between lovers, and a thing of beauty. To the beetle, a rose
is perhaps an entire territory, with places to hide (on the
underside of a leaf, invisible to aerial predators), hunt (in the
head of the flower where ant nymphs grow), and lay eggs (in the
joint of the leaf and stem). To the elephant, it is a thorn barely
detectable underfoot.
And to the dog, what is a rose? As we'll see,
this depends upon the construction of the dog, both in body and
brain. As it turns out, to the dog, a rose is neither a thing of
beauty nor a world unto itself. A rose is undistinguished from the
rest of the plant matter surrounding it—unless it has been urinated
upon by another dog, stepped on by another animal, or handled by
the dog's owner. Then it gains vivid interest, and becomes far more
significant to the dog than even the well-presented rose is to
us.
PUTTING OUR UMWELT CAPS ON
Discerning the salient elements in an animal's
world—his umwelt—is, in a sense, becoming an expert on the animal:
whether a tick, a dog, or a human being. And it will be our tool
for resolving the tension between what we think we know about dogs,
and what they are actually doing. Yet without anthropomorphisms we
would seem to have little vocabulary with which to describe their
perceived experience.
Understanding a dog's perspective—through
understanding his abilities, experience, and communication—provides
that vocabulary. But we can't translate it simply through an
introspection that brings our own umwelt along. Most of us are not
excellent smellers; to imagine being a smeller, we have to do more
than just think on it. That kind of introspective exercise only
works when paired with an understanding of how profound the
difference in umwelt is between us and another animal.
We can glimpse this by "acting into" the
umwelt of another animal, trying to embody the animal—mindful of
the constraints our sensory system places on our ability to truly
do so. Spending an afternoon at the height of a dog is surprising.
Smelling (even with our impoverished schnozes) every object we come
across in a day closely and deeply yields a new dimension on
otherwise familiar things. As you read this, try attending to all
the sounds in the room you are in now that you have become
accustomed to and usually tune out. With attention I suddenly hear
the fan behind me, a beeping truck heading in reverse, the
murmurations of a crowd of voices entering the building downstairs;
someone adjusts their body in a wooden chair, my heart beats, I
swallow, a page is turned. Were my hearing keener, I might notice
the scratch of pen on paper across the room; the sound of a plant
stretching in growth; the ultrasonic cries of the population of
insects always underfoot. Might these noises be in the foreground
in another animal's sensory universe?
THE
MEANING OF THINGS
Even the objects in a room are not, in some sense, the same objects to another animal. A dog looking around a room does not think he is surrounded by human things; he sees dog things. What we think an object is for, or what it makes us think of, may or may not match the dog's idea of the object's function or meaning. Objects are defined by how you can act upon them: what von Uexküll calls their functional tones—as though an object's use rings bell-like when you set eyes on it. A dog may be indifferent to chairs, but if trained to jump on one, he learns that the chair has a sitting tone: it can be sat upon. Later, the dog might himself decide that other objects have a sitting tone: a sofa, a pile of pillows, the lap of a person on the floor. But other things that we identify as chairlike are not so seen by dogs: stools, tables, arms of couches. Stools and tables are in some other category of objects: obstacles, perhaps, in their path toward the eating tone of the kitchen.
Here we begin to see how the dog and the human
overlap in our worldviews, and how we differ. A good many objects
in the world have an eating tone to the dog—probably many more than
we see as such. Feces just aren't menu items for us; dogs disagree.
Dogs may have tones that we don't have at all—rolling tones, say: things that one might merrily
roll in. Unless we are particularly playful or young, our list of
rolling-tone objects is small to nil. And plenty of ordinary
objects that have very specific meanings to us—forks, knives,
hammers, pushpins, fans, clocks, on and on—have little or no
meaning to dogs. To a dog, a hammer doesn't exist. A dog doesn't
act with or on a hammer, so it has no significance to a dog. At
least, not unless it overlaps with some other, meaningful object:
it is wielded by a loved person; it is urinated on by the cute dog
down the street; its dense wooden handle can be chewed like a
stick.
A clash of umwelts occurs when dog meets
human, and it tends to result in people misunderstanding what their
dog is doing. They aren't seeing the world from the dog's
perspective: the way he sees it. For instance, dog owners commonly
insist, in grave tones, that a dog is never to lie on the bed. To
drum in the seriousness of this dictum, this owner may go out and
purchase what a pillow manufacturer has decided to label a "dog
bed," and place it on the floor. The dog will be encouraged to come
and lie on this special bed, the non-forbidden bed. The dog
typically will do so, reluctantly. And thus one might feel
satisfied: another dog-human interaction successful!
But is it so? Many days I returned home to
find a warm, rumpled pile of sheets on my bed where either the
wagging dog who greeted me at the door, or some unseen sleepy
intruder, recently lay. We have no trouble seeing the meaning of
the beds to the human: the very names of the objects make the
situation clear. The big bed is for people; the dog bed is for
dogs. Human beds represent relaxation, may be expensively outfitted
with specially chosen sheets, and display all manner of fluffed
pillows; the dog bed is a place we would never think to sit, is
(relatively) inexpensive, and is more likely to be adorned with
chew toys than with pillows. What about to the dog? Initially,
there's not much difference between the beds—except, perhaps, that
our bed is infinitely more desirable. Our beds smell like us, while
the dog bed smells like whatever material the dog bed manufacturer
had lying around (or, worse, cedar chips—overwhelming perfume to a
dog but pleasant to us). And our beds are where we are: where we spend idle time, maybe shedding
crumbs and clothes. The dog's preference? Indisputably our bed. The
dog does not know all the things about the bed that make it such a
glaringly different object to us. He may, indeed, come to learn
that there is something different about the bed—by getting
repeatedly scolded for lying on it. Even then, what the dog knows
is less "human bed" versus "dog bed" but "thing one gets yelled at
for being on" versus "thing one does not get yelled at for being
on."
In the dog umwelt beds have no special
functional tone. Dogs sleep and rest where they can, not on objects
designated by people for those purposes. There may be a functional
tone for places to sleep: dogs prefer places that allow them to lie
down fully, where the temperature is desirable, where there are
other members of their troop or family around, and where they are
safe. Any flattish surface in your home satisfies these conditions.
Make one fit these criteria, and your dog will probably find it
just as desirable as your big, comfy human bed.
ASKING DOGS
To bolster our claims about the experience or
mind of a dog, we will learn how to ask the dog if we're right. The
trouble, of course, with asking a dog if he is happy or depressed
is not that the question makes no sense. It's that we are very poor
at understanding his response. We're made terribly lazy by
language. I might guess at the reasons behind my friend's
recalcitrant, standoffish behavior for weeks, forming elaborate,
psychologically complex descriptions of what her actions indicate
about what she thinks I meant on some fraught occasion. But my best
strategy by leaps is to simply ask her. She'll tell me. Dogs, on
the other hand, never answer in the way we'd hope: by replying in
sentences, well punctuated and with italicized emphases. Still, if
we look, they have plainly answered.
For instance, is a dog who watches you with a
sigh as you prepare to leave for work depressed? Are dogs left at
home all day pessimistic? Bored? Or just exhaling idly, preparing
for a nap?
Looking at behavior to learn about an animal's
mental experience is precisely the idea behind some cleverly
designed recent experiments. The researchers used not dogs, but
that shopworn research subject, the laboratory rat. The behavior of
rats in cages may be the single largest contributor to the corpus
of psychological knowledge. In most cases, the rat itself is not of
interest: the research isn't about rats per se. Surprisingly, it's
about humans. The notion is that rats learn and remember by using
some of the same mechanisms that humans use—but rats are easier to
keep in tiny boxes and subject to restricted stimuli in the hopes
of getting a response. And the millions of responses by millions of
laboratory rats, Rattus norvegicus,
have greatly informed our understanding of human
psychology.
But the rats themselves are intrinsically
interesting as well. People who work with rats in laboratories
sometimes describe their animals' "depression" or their exuberant
natures. Some rats seem lazy, some are cheery; some pessimistic,
some optimistic. The researchers took two of these
characterizations—pessimism and optimism—and gave them operational
definitions: definitions in terms of behavior that allow us to
determine whether real differences in the rats can be seen. Instead
of simply extrapolating from how humans look when pessimistic, we
can ask how a pessimistic rat might be distinguished by its
behavior from an optimistic one.
Thus, the rats' behavior was examined not as a
mirror to our own but as indicating something about … rats: about
rat preference and rat emotions. Their subjects were placed in
tightly restricted environments: some were "unpredictable"
environments, where the bedding, cage mates, and the light and dark
schedule were always changing; others were stable, predictable
environments. The experimental design took advantage of the fact
that, hanging out in their cages with little to do, rats quickly
learn to associate new events with simultaneously occurring
phenomena. In this case, a particular pitch was played over
speakers into the cages of the rats. It was a prompt to press a
lever: the lever triggered the arrival of a pellet of food. When a
different pitch was played and the rats pressed the lever, they
were greeted with an unpleasant sound and no food. These rats,
reliably like lab rats before them, quickly learned the
association. They raced over to the food-dispensing lever only when
the good-harbinger sound appeared, like young children rallied by
the jingle of an ice-cream truck. All of the rats learned this
easily. But when the rats were played a new sound, one between the
two learned pitches, what the researchers found was that the rats'
environment mattered. Those who had been housed in a predictable
environment interpreted the new sound to mean food; those in unstable environments did
not.
These rats had learned optimism or pessimism
about the world. To watch the rats in the predictable environments
jump with alacrity at every new sound is to see optimism in action.
Small changes in the environment were enough to prompt a large
change in outlook. Rat lab workers' intuitions about the mood of
their charges may be spot-on.
We can subject our intuitions about dogs to
the same kind of analysis. For any anthropomorphism we use to
describe our dogs, we can ask two questions: One, is there a
natural behavior this action might have evolved from? And two, what
would that anthropomorphic claim amount to if we deconstructed
it?
DOG
KISSES
Licks are Pump's way of making contact, her
hand outstretched for me. She greets me home with licks at my face
as I bend to pet her; I get waking licks on my hand as I nap in a
chair; she licks my legs thoroughly clean of salt after a run;
sitting beside me, she pins my hand with her front leg and pushes
open my fist to lick the soft warm flesh of my palm. I adore her
licks.
I frequently hear dog owners verify their dogs' love of them through the kisses delivered upon them when they return home. These "kisses" are licks: slobbery licks to the face; focused, exhaustive licking of the hand; solemn tongue-polishing of a limb. I confess that I treat Pump's licks as a sign of affection. "Affection" and "love" are not just the recent constructs of a society that treats pets as little people, to be shod in shoes in bad weather, dressed up for Halloween, and indulged with spa days. Before there was any such thing as a doggy day care, Charles Darwin (who I feel confident never dressed up his pup as a witch or goblin) wrote of receiving lick-kisses from his dogs. He was certain of their meaning: dogs have, he wrote, a "striking way of exhibiting their affection, namely, by licking the hands or faces of their masters." Was Darwin right? The kisses feel affectionate to me, but are they gestures of affection to the dog?
First, the bad news: researchers of wild
canids—wolves, coyotes, foxes, and other wild dogs—report that
puppies lick the face and muzzle of their mother when she returns
from a hunt to her den—in order to get her to regurgitate for them.
Licking around the mouth seems to be the cue that stimulates her to
vomit up some nicely partially digested meat. How disappointed Pump
must be that not a single time have I regurgitated half-eaten
rabbit flesh for her.
Furthermore, our mouths taste great to dogs.
Like wolves and humans, dogs have taste receptors for salty, sweet,
bitter, sour, and even umami, the earthy, mushroomy-seaweedy flavor
captured in the flavor-heightening monosodium glutamate. Their
perception of sweetness is processed slightly differently than
ours, in that salt enhances the experience of sweet tastes. The
sweet receptors are particularly abundant in dogs, although some
sweeteners—sucrose and fructose—activate the receptors more than
others, such as glucose. This could be adaptive in an omnivore like
the dog, for whom it pays to distinguish between ripe and non-ripe
plants and fruit. Interestingly, even pure salt doesn't kick-start
the so-called salt receptors on the tongue and the roof of the
mouth in dogs the way it does in humans. (There's some disagreement
whether dogs have salt-specific receptors at all.) But it didn't
take long reflecting on her behavior for me to realize that Pump's
licks to my face often correlated with my face having just overseen
the ingestion of a good amount of food.
Now the good news: as a result of this
functional use of mouth licking—"kisses" to you and me—the behavior
has become a ritualized greeting. In other words, it no longer
serves only the function of asking for food; now it is used to say
hello. Dogs and wolves muzzle-lick simply to welcome another dog
back home, and to get an olfactory report of where the homecomer
has been or what he has done. Mothers not only clean their pups by
licking, they often give a few darting licks when reuniting after
even a brief time apart. A younger or timid dog may lick the
muzzle, or muzzle vicinity, of a bigger, threatening dog to appease
him. Familiar dogs may exchange licks when meeting at their ends of
their respective leashes on the street. It may serve as a way to
confirm, through smell, that this dog storming toward them is who
they think he is. Since these "greeting licks" are often
accompanied by wagging tails, mouths opened playfully, and general
excitement, it is not a stretch to say that the licks are a way to
express happiness that you have returned.
DOGOLOGIST
I still talk about Pump's looking "knowingly," or feeling content or capricious. These are words that capture something to me. But I have no illusion that they map to her experience. And I still adore her licks; but I also adore knowing what they mean to her rather than just what they mean to me.
By imagining the umwelt of dogs, we'll be able
to deconstruct other anthropomorphisms—of our dog's guilt at
chewing a shoe; of a pup's revenge wrought on your new Hermès
scarf—and reconstruct them with the dog's understanding in mind.
Trying to understand a dog's perspective is like being an
anthropologist in a foreign land—one peopled entirely by dogs. A
perfect translation of every wag and woof may elude us, but simply
looking closely will reveal a surprising amount. So let's look
closely at what the natives do.
In the following chapters we will consider the
many dimensions contributing to a dog's umwelt. The first dimension
is historical: how dogs came from wolves, and how they are and are
not wolflike. The choices we've made in breeding dogs led to some
intentional designs and some unintended consequences. The next
dimension comes from anatomy: the dog's sensory capacity. We need
to appreciate what the dog smells, sees, and hears … and if there
are other means by which to sense the world. We must imagine the
view from two feet off the ground, and from behind such a snout.
Finally, the body of the dog leads us to the brain of the dog.
We'll look at the dog's cognitive abilities, the knowledge of which
can help us to translate their behavior. Together, these dimensions
combine to provide answers to the questions of what dogs think,
know, and understand. Ultimately they will serve as scientific
building blocks for an informed imaginative leap inside of a dog:
halfway to being honorary dogs ourselves.
Belonging to the House
She's waiting at the threshold of the kitchen,
just beyond underfoot. Somehow Pump knows precisely where "out of
the kitchen" is. Here she sprawls, and when I bring food to the
table, she ducks in to retrieve the kitchen fallings. At the table,
she gets a little of everything—and she'll at least entertain even
the most unlikely offering, if only to loll it around in her mouth
before unceremoniously depositing it on the ground. She does not
like raisins. Nor tomatoes. She'll suffer a grape, if she manages
to split it into juicy halves with her side teeth—then
deliberating, as though managing a very big or tough object, and
masticating it. All carrot ends are for her. She takes the stems of
broccoli and asparagus and holds them gently, gazing at me for a
moment as if determining if anything else is coming before walking
to the rug to settle down for a gnaw.
Dog training books often insist that "a dog is
an animal": this is true but is not the whole truth. The dog is an
animal domesticated, a word that grew
from a root form meaning "belonging to the house." Dogs are animals
who belong around houses. Domestication is a variation of the
process of evolution, where the selector has been not just natural
forces but human ones, eventually intent on bringing dogs inside
their homes.
To understand what the dog is about we have to
understand from where he came. As a member of the Canidae family—all of whose members are called
canids—the domestic dog is distantly
related to coyotes and jackals, dingoes and dholes, foxes and wild
dogs.*
But he arose from just one ancient
Canidae line, animals most likely
resembling the contemporary gray wolf. When I see Pumpernickel
delicately spit out a raisin, though, I am not reminded of the
stark images of wolves in Wyoming downing a moose and yanking it
apart.† The existence of an animal who will patiently wait at the
kitchen door, and then ponderously consider a carrot stick, seems
at first glance irreconcilable with that of an animal whose primary
allegiance is to himself, whose affiliations are fraught with
tension and maintained by force.
Carrot-considerers arose out of moose killers
through the second source: us. Where nature blindly, uncaringly
"selects" traits that lead to the survival of their bearers,
ancestral humans have also selected traits—physical features and
behaviors—that have led not just to the survival, but to the
omnipresence of the modern dog, Canis
familiaris, among us. The animal's appearance, behavior,
preferences; his interest in us and attention to our attention:
these are largely the result of domestication. Present-day dog is a
well-designed creature. Only much of this design was utterly
unintentional.
HOW TO MAKE A DOG: STEP-BY-STEP INSTRUCTIONS
So you want to make a dog? There are just a
few ingredients. You'll need wolves, humans, a little interaction,
mutual tolerance. Mix thoroughly and wait, oh, a few thousand
years.
Or, if you're the Russian geneticist Dmitry
Belyaev, you simply find a group of captive foxes and start
selectively breeding them. In 1959, Belyaev began a project that
has greatly informed our best guesses as to what we believe the
earliest steps of domestication were. Instead of observing dogs and
extrapolating backward, he examined another social canid species
and propagated them forward. The silver fox in Siberia in the
mid-twentieth century was a small, wild animal that had become
popular with the fur trade. Kept in pens, bred for their choice fur
coats, particularly long and soft, the fox was not tamed but was
captive. What Belyaev made of them, with a much reduced recipe,
were not "dogs," but were surprisingly close to dogs.
Though Vulpes
vulpes, the silver fox, is distantly related to wolves and
dogs, it had never before been domesticated. Despite their
evolutionary relatedness, no canids are fully domesticated other
than the dog: domestication doesn't happen spontaneously. What
Belyaev showed was that it can happen quickly. Beginning with 130
foxes, he selectively chose and bred those that were the most
"tame," as he described it. What he really chose were those foxes
that were the least fearful of or aggressive toward people. The
foxes were caged, so aggression was minimal. Belyaev approached
each cage and invited the fox to eat some food out of his
hand.
Some bit at him; some hid. Some took the food,
reluctantly. Others took the food and also let themselves be
touched and patted without fleeing or snarling. Still others
accepted the food and even wagged and whimpered at the
experimenter, inviting rather than discouraging interaction. These
were the foxes Belyaev selected. By some normal variation in their
genetic code, these animals were naturally calmer around people,
even interested in people. None of them had been trained; all had
the same, minimal exposure to human caretakers, who fed them and
cleaned their bedding for their short lives.
These "tame" foxes were allowed to mate, and
their young were tested the same way. The tamest of those were
mated, when they were old enough; and their young; and their young.
Belyaev continued the work until his death, and the program has
continued since. After forty years, three-quarters of the
population of foxes were of a class the researchers called
"domesticated elite": not just accepting contact with people, but
drawn to it, "whimpering to attract attention and sniffing and
licking"… as dogs do. He had created a domesticated fox.
Later genomic mapping has revealed that forty
genes now differ between Belyaev's tame foxes and the wild silver
fox. Incredibly, by selecting for one behavioral trait, the genome
of the animal was changed in a half century. And with that genetic
change came a number of surprisingly familiar physical changes:
some of the later-generation foxes have multicolored, piebald
coats, recognizable in dog mutts everywhere. They have floppy ears
and tails that curl up and over their backs. Their heads are wider
and their snouts are shorter. They are improbably cute.
All these physical characteristics came along
for the ride, once a particular behavior was chosen and picked out.
The behavior is not what affects the body; instead, both are the
common result of a gene or set of genes. Single behaviors aren't
dictated by genes, but they are made more or less likely by them.
If someone's genetic makeup leads to having very high levels of a
stress hormone, for instance, it doesn't mean that they will be
stressed all the time. But it may mean that they have a lowered
threshold for having the classic stress response—a raised heart
rate and breathing rate, increased sweating, and so on—in some
contexts where someone else doesn't have a stress response. Let's
say this low-threshold character screams at her dog for barreling
into her at the dog park. Her screaming at the poor pup certainly
is not genetically obliged—genes don't know from dog parks, or even
pups—but her neurochemistry, created from her genes, facilitated it
happening when a situation presented itself.
So, too, with the doglike foxes. Given what
genes do,* even a small change in a gene—turning on slightly later
than it otherwise might, say—could change the likelihood of both
certain behaviors and certain forms of physical appearance.
Belyaev's foxes show that a few simple developmental differences
can have a wide-ranging effect: for instance, his foxes open their
eyes earlier and show their first fear responses later, more like
dogs than wild foxes. This gives them a longer early window for
bonding with a caretaker—such as a human experimenter in Siberia.
They play with each other even when they reach adulthood, perhaps
allowing for longer and more complex socialization. It is worth
noting that foxes diverged from wolves some ten to twelve million
years ago; yet in forty years' of selection they look domesticated.
The same perhaps could happen with other carnivores we take under
our wing and inside our houses. The genetic changes nudge them into
being doggy.